Autoscopy: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Goblin Face (talk | contribs)
No edit summary
Goblin Face (talk | contribs)
Line 9: Line 9:
* impression of seeing one's own body from this perspective (autoscopy).
* impression of seeing one's own body from this perspective (autoscopy).


Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, [[École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne]], [[Lausanne]], and Department of Neurology, University Hospital, Geneva, Switzerland, have reviewed some of the classical precipitating factors of autoscopy. These are sleep, drug abuse, and general anesthesia as well neurobiology. They have compared them with recent findings on neurological and neurocognitive mechanisms of the autoscopy. The reviewed data suggest that autoscopic experiences are due to functional disintegration of lower-level multisensory processing and abnormal higher-level self-processing at the [[temporoparietal junction]].<ref name="Blanke 2005"/>
Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, [[École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne]], [[Lausanne]], and Department of Neurology, University Hospital, Geneva, Switzerland, have reviewed some of the classical precipitating factors of autoscopy. These are sleep, drug abuse, and general anesthesia as well neurobiology. They have compared them with recent findings on neurological and neurocognitive mechanisms of the autoscopy. The reviewed data suggest that autoscopic experiences are due to functional disintegration of lower-level multisensory processing and abnormal higher-level self-processing at the [[temporoparietal junction]].<ref name="Blanke 2005"/><ref>Blanke, O; Castillo, V. (2007). ''Clinical neuroimaging in epileptic patients with autoscopic hallucinations and out-of-body experiences''. Epileptologie 24: 90–96</ref>


== Related disorders ==
== Related disorders ==

Revision as of 22:22, 2 May 2014

Autoscopy is the experience in which an individual perceives the surrounding environment from a different perspective, from a position outside of his or her own body.[1] Autoscopy comes from the ancient Greek αὐτός ("self") and σκοπός ("watcher").

Autoscopy has intrigued humankind from time immemorial and is abundant in the folklore, mythology, and spiritual narratives of most ancient and modern societies. Cases of autoscopy are commonly encountered in modern psychiatric practice.[2] According to neurological research, autoscopic experiences are hallucinations.[1][3]

Factors

Experiences - are characterized by the presence of the following three factors:

  • disembodiment, an apparent location of the self outside one's body;
  • impression of seeing the world from an elevated and distanced visuo-spatial perspective or extracorporeal, but egocentric visuo-spatial perspective;
  • impression of seeing one's own body from this perspective (autoscopy).

Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, and Department of Neurology, University Hospital, Geneva, Switzerland, have reviewed some of the classical precipitating factors of autoscopy. These are sleep, drug abuse, and general anesthesia as well neurobiology. They have compared them with recent findings on neurological and neurocognitive mechanisms of the autoscopy. The reviewed data suggest that autoscopic experiences are due to functional disintegration of lower-level multisensory processing and abnormal higher-level self-processing at the temporoparietal junction.[1][4]

Related disorders

Heautoscopy is a term used in psychiatry and neurology for the reduplicative hallucination of "seeing one's own body at a distance".[5] It can occur as a symptom in schizophrenia[6] and epilepsy. Heautoscopy is considered as one possible explanation for doppelgänger phenomena.[7]

Another related autoscopy disorder is known as negative autoscopy (or negative heautoscopy) a psychological phenomenon in which the sufferer does not see his or her reflection when looking in a mirror. Although the sufferer's image may be seen by others, he or she claims not to see it.[1]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d Blanke, O., Mohr, C.(2005). Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin Implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self consciousness. Brain Research Reviews 50: 184-199.
  2. ^ Dening, T. R., Berrios, G. E. (1994). Autoscopic phenomena. British Journal of Psychiatry 165: 808-817.
  3. ^ Brugger, P; Regard, M; Landis, T. (1997). Illusory reduplication of one’s own body: phenomenology and classification of autoscopic phenomena. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 2: 19-38.
  4. ^ Blanke, O; Castillo, V. (2007). Clinical neuroimaging in epileptic patients with autoscopic hallucinations and out-of-body experiences. Epileptologie 24: 90–96
  5. ^ Damas Mora JMR, Jenner FA, Eacott SE (1980). "On heautoscopy or the phenomenon of the double: Case presentation and review of the literature". Br J Med Psychol. 53 (1): 75–83. PMID 6989391.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  6. ^ Blackmore S (1986). "Out-of-Body Experiences in Schizophrenia: A Questionnaire Survey". Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 174 (10): 615–619. doi:10.1097/00005053-198610000-00006. PMID 3760852.
  7. ^ Brugger, P; Agosti, R; Regard, M; Wieser, H. G; Landis, T. (1994). Heautoscopy, epilepsy, and suicide. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgergy and Psychiatry 57: 838-839.

Further reading

  • Bhaskaran, R; Kumar, A; Nayar, K. K. (1990). Autoscopy in hemianopic field. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry: 53 1016-1017.
  • Brugger, P; Regard, M; Landis, T. (1996). Unilaterally felt ‘‘presences’’: the neuropsychiatry of one’s invisible doppelgänger. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology 9: 114–122.
  • Devinsky, O., Feldmann, E., Burrowes, K; Bromfield, E. (1989). Autoscopic phenomena with seizures. Archives of Neurology 46: 1080-1088.
  • Lukianowicz, N. (1958). Autoscopic phenomena. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 80: 199-220.

External links