Lev Vygotsky: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Revert to the revision prior to revision 686478945 dated 2015-10-19 10:31:14 by 195.194.248.2 using popups
Tag: Reverted
Undid revision 1124335583 by MrDemeanour (talk) Looks like an inadvertent tool based edit. Reverting way too far back.
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Use mdy dates|date=November 2022}}
{{Short description|Soviet psychologist (1896–1934)}}
{{POV|date=April 2019|talk=POV and COI tags}}
{{Infobox scientist
{{Infobox scientist
| name = Lev Vygotsky
| name = Lev Vygotsky
| image = Lev_Vygotsky.jpg
| image = Lev_Vygotsky.jpg
| image_size = 190px
| image_size =
| alt =
| alt =
| caption =
| caption =
| birth_date = {{Birth date|1896|11|19}}
| birth_name = Lev Simkhovich Výgodsky
| birth_date = {{Birth date|1896|11|17}}
| birth_place = [[Orsha]], [[Russian Empire]], now in [[Belarus]]
| birth_place = [[Orsha]], [[Russian Empire]], now in Belarus
| death_date = {{Death date and age|1934|06|11|1896|11|17}}
| death_date = {{Death date and age|1934|06|11|1896|11|17}}
| death_place = [[Moscow]], [[Soviet Union]]
| death_place = Moscow, [[Soviet Union]]
| residence =
| residence =
| citizenship =
| citizenship =
| nationality = Russian
| fields = [[Psychology]]
| fields = [[Psychology]]
| alma_mater = [[Imperial Moscow University|Imperial Moscow University (1917)]] (''unfinished'');<br/>Shaniavskii Moscow City People's University
| workplaces =
| thesis_title = The Psychology of Art
| alma_mater = [[Moscow State University]], [[Shaniavskii Open University]]
| thesis_year = 1925
| doctoral_advisor =
| workplaces = [[Moscow State University]]
| academic_advisors =
| doctoral_advisor =
| doctoral_students =
| academic_advisors =
| doctoral_students =
| notable_students = [[Alexander Luria]]
| notable_students = [[Alexander Luria]]
| known_for = [[Cultural-historical psychology]], [[Zone of proximal development]]
| known_for = [[Cultural-historical psychology]], [[zone of proximal development]], [[inner speech]]
| author_abbrev_bot =
| author_abbrev_bot =
| author_abbrev_zoo =
| author_abbrev_zoo =
| influences = [[Wilhelm von Humboldt]], [[Alexander Potebnia]], [[Alfred Adler]], [[Kurt Koffka]], [[Kurt Lewin]], [[Max Wertheimer]], [[Wolfgang Köhler]], [[Kurt Goldstein]]
| influences = [[Baruch Spinoza]], [[Wilhelm von Humboldt]], [[Alexander Potebnia]], [[Alfred Adler]], [[Kurt Koffka]], [[Kurt Lewin]], [[Max Wertheimer]], [[Wolfgang Köhler]], [[Kurt Goldstein]], [[Karl Marx]], [[Jean Piaget]]
| influenced = [[Vygotsky Circle]], [[Evald Ilyenkov]], [[Jean Piaget]]
| influenced = [[Vygotsky Circle]], [[Evald Ilyenkov]], [[Urie Bronfenbrenner]], [[Patricia McKinsey Crittenden]]
| awards =
| awards =
| signature = <!--(filename only)-->
| signature = <!--(filename only)-->
| signature_alt =
| signature_alt =
| footnotes =
| footnotes =
| spouse = Roza Noevna Vygodskaia (nee Smekhova)
| spouse = Roza Noevna Vygodskaia (née Smekhova)
}}
}}
'''Lev Semyonovich Vygotsky''' ({{lang-ru|Лев Семёнович Вы́готский or Выго́тский}} ''Lev Semyonovich Vygotskiy'', born Лев Симхович Выгодский ''Lev Simkhovich Vygodskiy'', {{OldStyleDate|November 17|1896|November 5}} – June 11, 1934) was a Soviet psychologist, the founder of a theory of human cultural and bio-social development commonly referred to as [[cultural-historical psychology]], and leader of the [[Vygotsky Circle]].
'''Lev Semyonovich Vygotsky''' ({{lang-ru|Лев Семёнович Выго́тский|p=vɨˈɡotskʲɪj}}; {{lang-be|Леў Сямёнавіч Выго́цкі|p=vɨˈɡotskʲɪj}}; {{OldStyleDate|November 17|1896|November 5}} – June 11, 1934) was a Soviet [[psychologist]], known for his work on [[Developmental psychology|psychological development in children]]. He published on a diverse range of subjects, and from multiple views as his perspective changed over the years. Among his students was [[Alexander Luria]] and [[Kharkov school of psychology|Kharkiv school of psychology]].


He is known for his concept of the [[zone of proximal development]] (ZPD): the distance between what a student (apprentice, new employee, etc.) can do on their own, and what they can accomplish with the support of someone more knowledgeable about the activity. Vygotsky saw the ZPD as a measure of skills that are in the process of maturing, as supplement to measures of development that only look at a learner's independent ability.
Vygotsky's main work was in developmental psychology, and he proposed a theory of the development of higher cognitive functions in children that saw reasoning as emerging through practical activity in a social environment. During the earlier period of his career he argued that the development of reasoning was mediated by signs and symbols, and therefore contingent on cultural practices and language as well as on universal cognitive processes.


Also influential are his works on the relationship between language and thought, the development of language, and a general theory of development through actions and relationships in a socio-cultural environment. This can be found in many of his essays.
Vygotsky also posited a concept of the [[zone of proximal development]], often understood to refer to the way in which the acquisition of new knowledge is dependent on previous learning, as well as the availability of instruction.


== Overview of scientific legacy ==
During his lifetime Vygotsky's theories were controversial within the Soviet Union. In the 1930s Vygotsky's ideas were introduced in the West where they remained virtually unknown until the 1970s when they became a central component of the development of new paradigms in developmental and educational psychology. While initially Vygotsky's theories were ignored in the West, they are today widely known, although scholars do not always agree with them, or agree about what he meant. The early 21st century has seen scholarly reevaluations of many of Vygotsky's central concepts and theories.<ref name="revBook">Yasnitsky, A. & van der Veer, R. (Eds.) (2015). [http://www.routledge.com/books/details/9781138887305/ Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies]. Routledge, ISBN 978-1-13-888730-5</ref> A ''[[Review of General Psychology]]'' survey, published in 2002, ranked Vygotsky as the 83rd most cited psychologist of the 20th century.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Haggbloom |first=Steven J. |last2=''et al.'' |title=The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century |journal=Review of General Psychology |volume=6 |issue=2 |year=2002 |pages=139–152 |doi=10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139 |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug02/eminent.aspx |first2=Renee |last3=Warnick |first3=Jason E. |last4=Jones |first4=Vinessa K. |last5=Yarbrough |first5=Gary L. |last6=Russell |first6=Tenea M. |last7=Borecky |first7=Chris M. |last8=McGahhey |first8=Reagan |last9=Powell |first9=John L., III| displayauthors = 8 }}</ref>
Despite his claim for a "new psychology" that he foresaw as a "science of the Superman" of the [[Communism|Communist future]],<ref name="vygScienceSuperman">Yasnitsky, A. (2018). [http://individual.utoronto.ca/yasnitsky/texts/Yasnitsky_2018_Questioning_bookPreview.pdf Vygotsky's science of Superman: from Utopia to concrete psychology]. In Yasnitsky, A. (Ed.). (2018). [https://www.routledge.com/Questioning-Vygotskys-Legacy-Scientific-Psychology-or-Heroic-Cult/Yasnitsky/p/book/9781138481268 Questioning Vygotsky's Legacy: Scientific Psychology or Heroic Cult]. London & New York: Routledge.</ref><ref name="spinuzziScienceSuperman">[https://clayspinuzzi.com/ Dr. Clay Spinuzzi blog], book review: [https://spinuzzi.blogspot.com/2017/06/reading-new-myth-new-world.html ''New Myth, New World: From Nietzsche to Stalinism'']</ref><ref name="spinuzziSuperhumansSupermediators">Spinuzzi, C. (2018). [https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9781351060639/chapters/10.4324/9781351060639-6 From superhumans to supermediators: Locating the extraordinary in CHAT]. In A. Yasnitsky (Ed.), Questioning Vygotsky's legacy: Scientific psychology or heroic cult (pp. 137–166). New York, NY: Routledge.</ref><ref name="handbook-consciousness">Zavershneva, E. (2014). The problem of consciousness in Vygotsky's cultural-historical psychology. In A. Yasnitsky, R. Van der Veer & M. Ferrari (Eds.), ''The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology'', 63-97. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press</ref> Vygotsky's main work was in [[developmental psychology]]. In order to fully understand the human mind, he believed one must understand its genesis. Consequently, the majority of his work involved the study of infant and child behavior, as well as the development of language acquisition (such as the importance of [[pointing]] and [[inner speech]]<ref>{{ cite journal |title=Inner Speech: Development, Cognitive Functions, Phenomenology, and Neurobiology |year=2015 |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=141 |issue=5 |pages=931–965 |last1=Alderson-Day |first1=Ben |last2=Fernyhough |first2=Charles |pmc=4538954 |pmid=26011789 |doi=10.1037/bul0000021 }}</ref>) and the development of concepts; now often referred to as [[Schema (psychology)|schemas]] or schemata.<ref>{{ cite book |title=Chapter 6: The Development of Scientific Concepts in Childhood |year=1932 |url=https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/words/ch06.htm |last=Vygotsky |first=L.S. ''Thought and Language''}} marxists.org</ref><ref>Oxford Reference: [http://www.oxfordreference.com/view/10.1093/oi/authority.20110803120305922 ''Vygotsky blocks'']</ref><ref>[https://vimeo.com/10689139 ''Paula Towsey on the Blocks Experiment (2008)''] Vimeo.com</ref>


Early in the psychological research period of his career (1920s), which focused upon [[Mechanism (philosophy)|mechanistic]] and [[Reductionism|reductionist]] "instrumental psychology" in many ways inspired by the work of [[Ivan Pavlov]] (his theory of "higher nervous activity") and [[Vladimir Bekhterev]] (and his "reflexologist" followers), Vygotsky argued that human psychological development could be formed through the use of meaningless (i.e., virtually random) signs that he viewed as the psychological equivalent of instrument use in human labor and industry.<ref name="revBook"/> It was later during the "[[Holism|holistic]]" period of his career (the first half of the 1930s) that Vygotsky rejected this earlier reductionist views on signs.
==Biography==
Lev Vygotsky was born in the town of [[Orsha]], in the [[Russian Empire]] (present-day [[Belarus]]) into a non-religious middle class [[Russian Jews|Russian Jewish]] family. His father was a banker. He was raised in the city of [[Gomel]], Belarus, where he obtained both public and private education. In 1913 Vygotsky was admitted to the [[Moscow State University]] through a "[[Jewish quota#Countries legislating limitations on the admission of Jewish students|Jewish Lottery]]" to meet a three percent Jewish student quota for entry in Moscow and Saint Petersburg universities.<ref name="Wertsch 1985">{{cite book|last=Wertsch|first=J. V.|title=Vygotsky and the social formation of mind.|year=1985|publisher=Harvard University Press|location=Cambridge, MA|isbn=0-674-94351-1}}(p. 5-6)</ref> There he studied law and, in parallel, he attended lectures at fully official, but privately funded and non degree granting "[[:ru:Московский городской народный университет имени А. Л. Шанявского|Shanyavskii People’s University]]".<ref>[http://encyclopedia2.thefreedictionary.com/Shaniavskii+University Shaniavskii University]</ref> His early interests were in the arts and he might have aspired to be a literary critic, fascinated with the [[Russian formalism|formalism]] of his time.


While Vygotsky never met [[Jean Piaget]], he had read a number of his works and agreed on some of his perspectives on learning.<ref name=":0">{{Cite book|last1=Hassard|first1=Jack|title=''The Art of Teaching Science: Inquiry and Innovation in Middle School and High School''|last2=Dias|first2=Michael|date=2013|publisher=Routledge|isbn=978-1-135-88999-9|location=Oxon}}</ref> At some point (around 1929–30), Vygotsky came to disagree with Piaget's understanding of learning and development, and held a different theoretical position from Piaget on the topic of inner speech; Piaget asserted that egocentric speech in children "dissolved away" as they matured, while Vygotsky maintained that egocentric speech became internalized, what we now call "inner speech".<ref>Vygotsky, L. S. & Luria, A., (1930) [https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1934/tool-symbol.htm Tool and symbol in child development] marxists.org</ref> However, in the early 1930s he radically changed his mind on Piaget's theory and openly praised him for his discovery of the social origin of children's speech, reasoning, and moral judgements. Piaget only read Vygotsky's work after his death.<ref name=":0" />
Upon graduation in 1917, Vygotsky returned to Gomel, where he lived after the [[October Revolution|October Socialist Revolution]] of 1917 happened. There is virtually no information about his life during the years of the German occupation and the Civil War until the Bolsheviks captured the town in 1919. Subsequently Vygotsky was an active participant of major social transformation under the Bolshevik rule and a fairly prominent representative of the Bolshevik government in Gomel from 1919 to 1923. For unclear reasons, around the early 1920s, he changed his birth name from Vygodskii (with "d") into Vygotskii (with middle "t") and his patronymic from original Jewish "Simkhovich" to Slavic "Semenovich".<ref>Б. Г. Мещеряков. [http://psyjournals.ru/kip/2007/n3/Meshcheryakov.shtml "Л. С. Выготский и его имя"]. Культурно-историческая психология №3/2007</ref>


Nearing the end of his life, Vygotsky's later work involved [[adolescent development]].<ref>Lev Vygotsky (1931) [https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1931/adolescent/ch10.htm marxists.org: Adolescent Pedagogy ''The development of thinking and concept formation in adolescence'']</ref> However, his most important and widely known contribution is his theory for the development of "higher psychological functions," which considers human psychological development as emerging through unification of interpersonal connections and actions taken within a given socio-cultural environment (i.e., language, culture, society, and tool-use). Vygotsky eventually came to dialogue with the mainstream Gestalt line of thought and adopted a more holistic approach to understanding development. Under the increasing influence of the holistic thinking of the scholars primarily associated with the German-American [[Gestalt psychology]] movement, Vygotsky adopted their views on "psychological systems" and—inspired by [[Kurt Lewin]]'s "[[field theory (psychology)|topological (and vector) psychology]]"—introduced the enigmatic construct of the "[[zone of proximal development]]". It was during this period that he identified the play of young children as their "leading activity", which he understood to be the main source of preschoolers' psychological development, and which he viewed as an expression of an inseparable unity of emotional, volitional, and cognitive development. At this time, Vygotsky fully revealed his long-time interest in the [[philosophy of Baruch Spinoza|philosophy of Spinoza]], who would remain one of his favorite thinkers throughout his life. A fervent [[Spinozism|Spinozist]] in many respects, Vygotsky was profoundly influenced by Spinoza's thought, largely in response to Spinoza's examinations concerning human emotion.<ref>Vygotsky, L. S. (1931–32) [https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1932/spinoza.htm ''On Spinoza''] marxists.org</ref><ref>
In January 1924, Vygotsky took part in the Second All-Russian Psychoneurological Congress in Leningrad. Soon thereafter, Vygotsky received an invitation to become a research fellow at the Psychological Institute in Moscow. Vygotsky moved to Moscow with new wife Roza Smekhova. He began his career at the Psychological Institute as a "staff scientist, second class".<ref name="Wertsch 1985g">{{cite book|last=Wertsch|first=J. V.|title=Vygotsky and the social formation of mind.|year=1985|publisher=Harvard University Press|location=Cambridge, MA|isbn=0-674-94351-1}} (p. 10)</ref><ref>Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). [https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/handle/1887/10170 Understanding Vygotsky. A quest for synthesis]. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.</ref>
* Vygotsky: "...My intellect has been shaped under the sign of Spinoza's words, and it has tried not to be astounded, not to laugh, not to cry, but to understand." (in his dissertation thesis ''Psychology of Art'') [original in Russian]
By the end of 1925, Vygotsky completed his dissertation in 1925 on "The Psychology of Art" (not published until the 1960s) and a book "Pedagogical Psychology" that was apparently created on the basis of lecture notes that he prepared back in Gomel as a psychology instructor at local educational establishments. In summer 1925 he made his first and only trip abroad to a London congress on the education of the deaf.<ref>van der Veer, R. & Zavershneva, E. (2011). To Moscow with Love: Partial Reconstruction of Vygotsky’s Trip to London. ''Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science'' 45(4), 458–474: [http://www.springerlink.com/content/375141xv6284506g/fulltext.html PDF], [http://www.springerlink.com/content/375141xv6284506g/fulltext.pdf ''pdf'']</ref> Upon return to the Soviet Union, he was hospitalized due to relapse of tuberculosis and, having miraculously survived, remained an invalid and out of job until the end of 1926.<ref>Завершнева Е.Ю. «Ключ к психологии человека»: комментарии к блокноту Л.С. Выготского из больницы «Захарьино» (1926 г.) // Вопр. психол. 2009. №3. С. 123—141</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. "The Key to Human Psychology". Commentary on L.S. Vygotsky’s Notebook from the Zakharino Hospital (1926). ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'' vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012</ref> His dissertation was accepted as the prerequisite of scholarly degree, which was awarded to Vygotsky in fall 1925 ''[[graduation in absentia|in absentia]]''.
* Vygotsky: "...From the great creations of Spinoza, as from distant stars, light takes several centuries to reach us. Only the psychology of the future will be able to realize the ideas of Spinoza." [original in Russian]
* Vygotsky: "...We cannot help but note that we have come to the same understanding of freedom and self-control that Spinoza developed in his 'Ethics'." (''Self-Control'', 1931) [original in Russian]
* Vygotsky: "...Spinoza's teaching contains specifically what is in neither of the two parts into which contemporary psychology of emotions has disintegrated: the unity of the causal explanation and the problem of the vital significance of human passions, the unity of descriptive and explanatory psychology of feelings. For this reason, Spinoza is closely connected with the most vital, the most critical news of the day for contemporary psychology of emotions, news of the day which prevails in it, determining the paroxysm of crisis that envelops it. The problems of Spinoza await their solution, without which tomorrow's day in our psychology is impossible." (''The Teaching about Emotions'', 1932) [original in Russian]</ref><ref>Kline, G.L. (ed.): ''Spinoza in Soviet Philosophy. A Series of Essays Selected and Translated and with an Introduction''. (New York: The Humanities Press, 1952)</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Maidansky | first1 = A. | year = 2003 | title = The Russian Spinozists | url = http://caute.ru/am/text/rse.htm | journal = Studies in East European Thought | volume = 5 | issue = 3| pages = 199–216 | doi = 10.1023/A:1024066221394 | s2cid = 169586377 }}</ref><ref>Derry, J. ''Vygotsky, Philosophy and Education''. (Chichester, West Sussex: Wiley Blackwell, 2013). Derry (2013, p. 85): "Vygotsky's understanding of free will derives from Spinoza. His work is peppered with references to Spinoza and, according to his childhood friend Semyon Dobkin, Spinoza was his favourite philosopher".</ref><ref>Secker, M.: ''Spinoza's Theory of Emotion in Relation to Vygotsky's Psychology and [[Antonio Damasio|Damasio]]'s Neuroscience''. (Ph.D. diss., [[University of East Anglia]], 2014)</ref><ref>[[Wolff-Michael Roth|Roth, W.-M.]]: ''The Mathematics of Mathematics: Thinking with the Late, [[Spinozist]] Vygotsky''. (Rotterdam: Sense Publishers, 2017)</ref><ref>Roth, W.-M.; Jornet, A.: ''Understanding Educational Psychology: A Late Vygotskian, Spinozist Approach''. (Dordrecht: Springer, 2017)</ref><ref>{{cite journal | doi = 10.1080/10749039.2018.1538379 | volume=25 | title=Introduction to symposium on Vygotsky and Spinoza | year=2018 | journal=Mind, Culture, and Activity | pages=340–345 | last1 = Jornet | first1 = A. | last2 = Cole | first2 = M.| issue=4 | s2cid=145032408 }}. Jornet & Cole (2018): "It has been known since the publication of ''Thought and Language'' in English that at the end of his life, Vygotsky turned to the ideas of Spinoza to overcome what he considered the shortcomings of his earlier theoretical ideas, bringing emotion to center stage in the process of development. Recent scholarship has made it clear that Spinoza was important from the beginning of Vygotsky's career. His doctoral thesis, ''The Psychology of Art'', opens with a quotation from Spinoza, and years later [[Aleksei N. Leontiev|Leont'ev]] (1997) made it clear in his introduction to Vygotsky's collected works that Vygotsky's interest in the philosophy of Spinoza began as early as his student years, and "would remain his favorite thinker for the rest of his life". Spinoza's lifelong influence on Vygotsky, however, has remained a relatively unexplored issue."</ref> As his work matured, Spinoza's thought became a more central visitation in Vygotsky's later work, increasingly focused on the issue of human emotion and its role in higher psychological functions and development that he largely omitted in his earlier work and utterly needed for creating a holistic psychological theory.


As early as the mid-1920s, Vygotsky's ideas were introduced in the West, but he remained virtually unknown until the early 1980s when the popularity among educators of the [[Constructivism (philosophy of education)|constructivist]] developmental psychology and educational theory of Jean Piaget (1896-1980) started to decline and, in contrast, Vygotsky's notion of the "zone of proximal development" became a central component of the development of "[[Social constructivism|social constructivist]]" turn in developmental and, primarily, educational psychology and practice. A ''[[Review of General Psychology]]'' study, published in 2002, ranked Vygotsky as the 83rd top psychologist of the twentieth century and the third (and the last) Russian on the top-100 list after [[Ivan Pavlov]] and Vygotsky's longtime collaborator [[Alexander Luria]].<ref>{{cite journal |title=The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century |journal=Review of General Psychology |volume=6 |issue=2 |year=2002 |pages=139–152 |doi=10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139 |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug02/eminent.aspx |last1=Haggbloom |first1=S.J. |last2=Warnick |first2=R. |last3=Warnick |first3=J.E. |last4=Jones |first4=V.K. |last5=Yarbrough |first5=G.L. |last6=Russell |first6=T.M. |last7=Borecky |first7=C.M. |last8=McGahhey |first8=R. |last9=Powell III |first9=J.L. |last10=Beavers |first10=J. |last11=Monte |first11=E. |citeseerx=10.1.1.586.1913 |s2cid=145668721 }}</ref>
After his release from hospital Vygotsky did theoretical and methodological work on the crisis in psychology, but never finished the draft of the manuscript and interrupted his work on it around mid-1927. The manuscript was later published with notable editorial interventions and distortions in 1982 and presented by the editors as one of the most important Vygotsky's works.<ref name="Zavershneva, E 2009">Zavershneva, E. 2009. Issledovanie rukopisi L.S. Vygotskogo "Istoricheskii smysl psikhologicheskogo krizisa" [Investigation of the original of Vygotsky's manuscript "Historical meaning of crisis in psychology"]. Voprosy psikhologii (6):119-137.</ref><ref name="ReferenceA">Завершнева Е.Ю. Исследование рукописи Л.С. Выготского "Исторический смысл психологического кризиса" // Вопросы психологии, 2009. №6, с. 119 - 138.</ref><ref name="ReferenceB">Zavershneva, E. Investigating the Manuscript of L.S. Vygotsky’s "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology". ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012</ref><ref name="ReferenceC">Завершнева Е.Ю., Осипов М.Е. Основные поправки к тексту «Исторический смысл психологического кризиса», опубликованному в 1982 г. в собрании сочинений Л.С. Выготского // Вопросы психологии, 2010. №1. С. 92—103</ref><ref>E. Iu. Zavershneva and M.E. Osipov. Primary Changes to the Version of "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology" Published in the Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012</ref> In this early manuscript, Vygotsky argued for the formation of a general psychology that could unite the naturalist objectivist strands of psychological science with the more philosophical approaches of Marxist orientation. However, he also harshly criticized those of his colleagues who attempted to build a "[[Marxism|Marxist]] Psychology" as an alternative to the naturalist and philosophical schools. He argued that if one wanted to build a truly Marxist Psychology, there were no shortcuts to be found by merely looking for applicable quotes in [[Karl Marx|Marx]]' writings. Rather one should look for a methodology that was in accordance with the Marxian spirit.<ref name="Kozulin1986">Kozulin, Alex. 1986. "Vygotsky in Context" in Vygotsky L. "Thought and Language", MIT Press. pp. xi - lvii</ref>


==Biography==
In 1926-30 Vygotsky worked on a research programme investigating the development of higher cognitive functions of logical memory, selective attention, decision making and language comprehension, from early forms of primal psychological functions. During this period he gathered a group of students including [[Alexander Luria]], [[Alexei Leontiev]] and several others. Vygotsky guided his students in researching this phenomenon from three different angles: The instrumental angle, which tried to understand the ways in which humans use objects as aides of mediation in memory and reasoning. A developmental approach, focusing on how children acquire the higher cognitive functions during development. And a culture-historical approach, studying the ways in which forms of mediation and developmental trajectories are shaped by different social and cultural patterns of interaction.<ref name="Kozulin1986"/>
Lev Semionovich Vygotsky ({{lang-rus|Лев Семёнович Выго́тский|p=vɨˈɡotskʲɪj}}; {{OldStyleDate|November 17|1896|November 5}} – June 11, 1934) was born to the Vygodskii family in the town of [[Orsha]], Belarus (then belonging to the [[Russian Empire]]) into a non-religious middle-class family of [[Russian Jews|Russian Jewish]] extraction.<ref>{{Cite book|last=Pound|first=L.|title=How Children Learn|date=2019|publisher=Andrews UK Limited|isbn=978-1-909280-73-1|location=London|pages=51|language=en|edition=New}}</ref> His father Simkha Vygodskii was a banker.


Vygodskii was raised in the city of [[Gomel]], where he was homeschooled until 1911 and then obtained a formal degree with distinction in a private Jewish gymnasium, which allowed him entrance to a university. In 1913 Vygodskii was admitted to the [[Moscow University]] by mere ballot through a "[[Jewish quota#Countries legislating limitations on the admission of Jewish students|Jewish Lottery]]": at the time a three percent Jewish student quota was administered for entry in Moscow and Saint Petersburg universities. He had interest in humanities and social sciences, but at the insistence of his parents he applied to the medical school in Moscow University. During the first semester of study he transferred to the law school. There he studied law and, in parallel, he attended lectures at Shaniavskii University.
In early 1930s Vygotsky experienced deep crisis, personal and theoretical, and after a period of massive self-criticism made an attempt at a radical revision of his theory. The work of the representatives of the [[Gestalt psychology]] and other holistic scholars was instrumental in this theoretical shift. In 1932-1934 Vygotsky was aiming at establishing a psychological theory of consciousness, but this theory because of his death remained only in a very sketchy and unfinished form.


Vygodskii's early interests were in the arts and, primarily, in the topics of the history of the Jewish people, the tradition, culture and Jewish identity. In contrast, during this period he was highly critical of the ideas of both [[socialism]] and [[Zionism]], and proposed the solution of the "Jewish question" by return to the traditional Jewish Orthodoxy. His own academics, however, included a wide field of studies including linguistics, psychology, and philosophy.
==Scientific legacy==
Vygotsky was a pioneering psychologist and his major works span six separate volumes, written over roughly 10 years, from ''Psychology of Art'' (1925) to ''Thought and Language'' [or ''Thinking and Speech''] (1934). Vygotsky's interests in the fields of [[developmental psychology]], [[child development]], and [[education]] were extremely diverse. His philosophical framework includes insightful interpretations of the cognitive role of mediation tools, as well as the re-interpretation of well-known concepts in psychology such as [[internalization]] of knowledge. Vygotsky introduced the notion of [[zone of proximal development]], an innovative metaphor capable of describing the potential of human cognitive development. His work covered such diverse topics as the origin and the [[psychology of art]], development of higher [[mental function]]s, [[philosophy of science]] and [[Methodology|methodology of psychological research]], the relation between [[learning]] and [[Developmental psychology|human development]], concept formation, interrelation between [[language and thought]] development, play as a psychological phenomenon, [[learning disabilities]], and abnormal human development (aka ''defectology''). His scientific thinking underwent several major transformations throughout his career, but generally Vygotsky's legacy can be divided into two fairly distinct periods{{Citation needed|date=March 2015}} and the transitional phase between the two during which Vygotsky experienced the crisis in his theory and personal life. These are the [[mechanism (philosophy)|mechanistic]] "instrumental" period of the 1920s, integrative "[[Holism|holistic]]" period of the 1930s, and the transitional years of, roughly, 1929-1931. Each of these periods is characterized by its distinct themes and theoretical innovations.


Interrupted by the [[October Revolution|October]] [[Bolshevik]] uprising in 1917, Vygotsky never completed his formal studies at the [[Imperial Moscow University]], and thus, he never obtained a degree. Following these events, he left Moscow and eventually returned to Gomel. There is virtually no information about his life when Gomel was under [[German Empire|German]] [[Treaty of Brest-Litovsk|occupation]] (administratively belonging to the [[Ukrainian State]] at the time) during [[World War I]], until the [[Bolsheviks]] captured the city in 1919. After that, he was an active participant of major social transformation under the Bolshevik (Communist) rule and a fairly prominent representative of the Bolshevik government in Gomel from 1919 to 1923. By the early 1920s, as reflected in his journalistic publications of the time, he informally changed his Jewish-sounding birth name, 'Lev Símkhovich Výgodskii' ({{lang-rus|Лев Си́мхович Вы́годский}}), with the surname becoming ''Vygótskii'' and the [[Russian patronymic|patronymic]] 'Símkhovich' becoming the Slavic 'Semiónovich'. It was under this pen-name that the fame subsequently came to him. His daughters (subsequently born in 1925 and 1930) and other relatives, though, preserved their original family name 'Vygodskii'. The traditional English spelling of his last name nowadays is 'Vygotsky'.<ref name="bioBook">Yasnitsky, A. (2018). [https://www.routledge.com/Vygotsky-An-Intellectual-Biography/Yasnitsky/p/book/9781138806740 Vygotsky: An Intellectual Biography]. London and New York: Routledge [http://individual.utoronto.ca/yasnitsky/texts/Yasnitsky_2018_Vygotsky_bookPreview.pdf BOOK PREVIEW]</ref>
==="Instrumental" period (1920s)===
{{expand section|date=June 2013}}


In January 1924, Vygotsky took part in the Second All-Russian Psychoneurological Congress in [[Petrograd]] (soon thereafter renamed [[Leningrad]]). After the Congress, Vygotsky met with [[Alexander Luria]] and with his help received an invitation to become a research fellow at the Psychological Institute in Moscow which was under the direction of [[Konstantin Kornilov]]. Vygotsky moved to Moscow with his new wife, Roza Smekhova. He began his career at the Psychological Institute as a "staff scientist, second class".<ref>Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). [https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/handle/1887/10170 Understanding Vygotsky. A quest for synthesis]. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.</ref> He also became a secondary teacher, covering a period marked by his interest in the processes of learning and the role of language in learning.<ref>{{Cite book|last1=Nutbrown|first1=Cathy|title=Early Childhood Education: History, Philosophy and Experience|last2=Clough|first2=Peter|last3=Selbie|first3=Philip|date=2008|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4129-4497-7|location=Thousand Oaks, CA|pages=57}}</ref>
====Cultural mediation and internalization====
Vygotsky studied child development and the significant roles of [[cultural mediation]] and [[interpersonal communication]]. He observed how higher mental functions developed through these interactions also represented the shared knowledge of a culture. This process is known as internalization.<ref>Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press</ref>


By the end of 1925, Vygotsky completed his dissertation titled "The Psychology of Art", that was not published until the 1960s, and a book titled "Pedagogical Psychology", that apparently was created on the basis of lecture notes that he prepared in Gomel while he was a psychology instructor at local educational establishments. In the summer of 1925 he made his first and only trip abroad to a London congress on the education of the deaf.<ref>van der Veer, R. & Zavershneva, E. (2011). To Moscow with Love: Partial Reconstruction of Vygotsky's Trip to London. ''Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science'' 45(4), 458–474: [https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs12124-011-9173-8 PDF], [https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs12124-011-9173-8 ''pdf'']</ref> Upon return to the Soviet Union, he was hospitalized due to [[tuberculosis]] and, having miraculously survived, would remain an invalid and out of work until the end of 1926.<ref>Завершнева Е.Ю. «Ключ к психологии человека»: комментарии к блокноту Л.С. Выготского из больницы «Захарьино» (1926 г.) // Вопр. психол. 2009. №3. С. 123—141</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. "The Key to Human Psychology". Commentary on L.S. Vygotsky's Notebook from the Zakharino Hospital (1926). ''[[Journal of Russian and East European Psychology]]'' 50(4), July–August 2012</ref> His dissertation was accepted as the prerequisite of a scholarly degree, which was awarded to Vygotsky in autumn 1925 ''[[graduation in absentia|in absentia]]''.
''Internalization'' can be understood in one respect as "knowing how". For example, riding a bicycle or pouring a cup of milk are tools of the society and are initially outside and beyond the child. The mastery of these skills occurs through the activity of the child within society. A further aspect of internalization is ''appropriation'', in which the child takes a tool and makes it his own, perhaps using it in a way unique to himself. Internalizing the use of a pencil allows the child to use it very much for his own ends rather than drawing exactly what others in society have drawn previously.


After his release from the hospital, Vygotsky did theoretical and methodological work on the crisis in psychology, but never finished the draft of the manuscript and interrupted his work on it around mid-1927. The manuscript was published later with notable editorial interventions and distortions in 1982 and was presented by the editors as one of the most important of Vygotsky's works.<ref name="Zavershneva, E 2009">Zavershneva, E. 2009. Issledovanie rukopisi L.S. Vygotskogo "Istoricheskii smysl psikhologicheskogo krizisa" [Investigation of the original of Vygotsky's manuscript "Historical meaning of crisis in psychology"]. Voprosy psikhologii (6):119-137.</ref><ref name="ReferenceA">Завершнева Е.Ю. Исследование рукописи Л.С. Выготского "Исторический смысл психологического кризиса" // Вопросы психологии, 2009. №6, с. 119 - 138.</ref><ref name="ReferenceB">Zavershneva, E. Investigating the Manuscript of L.S. Vygotsky's "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology". ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', 50(4), July–August 2012</ref><ref name="ReferenceC">Завершнева Е.Ю., Осипов М.Е. Основные поправки к тексту «Исторический смысл психологического кризиса», опубликованному в 1982 г. в собрании сочинений Л.С. Выготского // Вопросы психологии, 2010. №1. С. 92—103</ref><ref>E. Iu. Zavershneva and M. E. Osipov. Primary Changes to the Version of "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology" Published in the Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', vol. 50(4), July–August 2012</ref> In this early manuscript, Vygotsky argued for the formation of a general psychology that could unite the naturalist objectivist strands of psychological science with the more philosophical approaches of Marxist orientation. However, he also harshly criticized those of his colleagues who attempted to build a "[[Marxism|Marxist]] Psychology" as an alternative to the naturalist and philosophical schools. He argued that if one wanted to build a truly Marxist Psychology, there were no shortcuts to be found by merely looking for applicable quotes in the writings of [[Karl Marx|Marx]]. Rather one should look for a methodology that was in accordance with the Marxian spirit.<ref name="Kozulin1986">Kozulin, Alex. 1986. "Vygotsky in Context" in Vygotsky L. "Thought and Language", MIT Press. pp. xi - lvii</ref>
===The period of crisis, criticism, and self-criticism (1929–1932)===
{{expand section|date=June 2013}}
In 1930s Vygotsky was engaged in massive reconstruction of his theory of his "instrumental" period of the 1920s. Around 1929-1930 he realized numerous deficiencies and imperfections of the earlier work of the Vygotsky Circle and criticized it on a number of occasions: in 1929,<ref>Cf. self-criticism of 1929: "I am revising the s[econd] part of "monkey"[i.e., the book Ape, primitive, and child]. Alas! The f[irst] chapter is written wholly according to the Freudianists [...]; then the impenetrable Piaget is turned into an absolute beyond all measure; instrument and sign are mixed together even more, and so on and so forth . This is not the fault of A.R. [Luria] personally, but of the entire "epoch" of our thinking. We need to put a stop to this unrelentingly. [...] Let there be the most rigorous, monastic regime of thought; ideological seclusion, if necessary. And let us demand the same of others. Let us explain that studying cultural psychology is no joke, not something to do at odd moments or among other things, and not grounds for every new person’s own conjectures". In: Vygotsky, L. S. (2007). Letters to students and colleagues. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', 45(2), 11-60. {{DOI|10.2753/RPO1061-0405450201}}</ref> 1930,<ref>Cf. self-criticism of 1930: "In the process of development, and in the historical development in particular, it is not so much the functions which change (these we mistakenly studies before). Their structure and the system of their development remain the same. What is changed and modified are rather the relationships, the links between the functions. New constellations emerge which were unknown in the preceding stage". In: Vygotsky, L. S. (1930/1997). On psychological systems. In R. W. Rieber & J. Wollock (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 3. Problems of the Theory and History of Psychology, pp. 91-108). New York: Plenum Press</ref> in 1931,<ref>From the letter to A.R. Luria, from Moscow, June 12, 1931: "I am still beset with thousands of petty chores. The fruitlessness of what I do greatly distresses me. My scientific thinking is going off into the realm of fantasy, and I cannot think things through in a realistic way to the end. Nothing is going right: I am doing the wrong things, writing the wrong things, saying the wrong things. A fundamental reorganization is called for—and this time I am going to carry it out." In: Vygotsky, L. S. (2007). Letters to students and colleagues. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', 45(2), 11-60. doi:10.2753/RPO1061-0405450201, p. 36</ref> and in 1932.<ref>Vygotsky, archival document of mid-1932 titled "Consciousness without word": "'''Our deficiency is not a deficiency of facts, but the untenability of the theory''': in the analysis of our crisis this is the main difficulty, but not a departure from facts. '''This is ''contra'' A[.]N.[Leontiev.]''' '''Consequently''': salvation is not in the facts but in the theory. We introduced the systemic point of view '''too late'''... '''Now''' I understand all this more deeply" (Zavershneva, 2010b, p. 54)</ref> Specifically, Vygotsky criticized his earlier idea of radical separation between the "lower" and "higher" psychological functions and, around 1932, appears to abandon it.<ref>Vygotsky in his presentation of December, 1932, a year and half before his death: "1. The necessity of a new stage of inquiry does not stem from the fact that a new thought has occurred to me or a new idea has caught my interest, but from the necessity of developing the research itself—new facts prod me into searching for new and more intricate explanations. The narrowness, bias, and schematism of the old mindset led us to the wrong assessment of the essential principles that we mistook for the secondary ones: interfunctional connections. We focused attention on the sign (on the tool) to the detriment of the operation with it, representing it as something simple, which goes through three phases: magical—external—internal. But the knot is external and the teenager’s diary is external. Hence we have a sea of poorly explained facts and a desire to delve more deeply into the facts, i.e., to evaluate them theoretically in a different way.
2. The higher and lower functions are not constructed in two tiers: their number and names do not match. But our previous understanding was not right, either[, according to which] a higher function is the mastery of the lower ([e.g.,] voluntary attention is the subordination to it of involuntary attention) because this means exactly—in two tiers". Vygotsky’s record titled "Symposium, December 4, 1932", see in Zavershneva, E. 2010b. The Vygotsky Family Archive: New Findings. Notebooks, Notes, and Scientific Journals of L.S. Vygotsky (1912–1934). ''Journal of the Russian and East European Psychology'' 48 (1):34-60, pp. 41-42</ref>


From 1926 to 1930, Vygotsky worked on a research program investigating the development of higher cognitive functions of logical memory, selective attention, decision making, and language comprehension, from early forms of primal psychological functions. During this period he gathered a group of collaborators including Alexander Luria, Boris Varshava, [[Alexei Leontiev]], [[Leonid Zankov]], and several others. Vygotsky guided his students in researching this phenomenon from three different perspectives:
This Vygotsky's self-criticism was complemented by external criticism for a number of issues, including the separation between the "higher" and the "lower" psychological functions, impracticality and inapplicability of his theory in social practices (such as industry or education) during the time of rapid social change, and vulgar Marxist interpretation of human psychological processes. Critics also pointed to his overemphasis on the role of language and, on the other hand, the ignorance of the emotional factors in human development. Major figures in Soviet psychology such as [[Sergei Rubinstein]] criticized Vygotsky's notion of mediation and its development in the works of students. Following criticism and in response to generous offer from the highest officials in Soviet Ukraine, a major group of Vygotsky's associates, the members of the [[Vygotsky Circle]], including [[Alexander Luria|Luria]], [[Mark Lebedinsky]], and [[Aleksei N. Leontiev|Leontiev]], moved from Moscow to Ukraine to establish the [[Kharkov School of Psychology|Kharkov school of psychology]]. In the second half of the 1930s, Vygotsky would be yet again criticized for his involvement in the cross-disciplinary study of the child known as [[paedology]] and uncritical borrowings from contemporary "bourgeois" science. Considerable critique came from the alleged Vygotsky's followers, such as Leontiev and members of his research group in Kharkov. Much of this early criticism was later discarded by these Vygotskian scholars themselves.
* The instrumental approach, which aimed to understand the ways humans use objects as mediation aids in memory and reasoning.
* A developmental approach, focused on how children acquire higher cognitive functions during development
* A culture-historical approach, studying how social and cultural patterns of interaction shape forms of mediation and developmental trajectories <ref name="Kozulin1986"/>


In the early 1930s, Vygotsky experienced deep crises, both personal and theoretical, and after a period of massive self-criticism, he made an attempt at a radical revision of his theory. The work of the representatives of the [[Gestalt psychology]] and other holistic scholars was instrumental in this theoretical shift. In 1932–1934, Vygotsky aimed to establish a psychological theory of consciousness, but because of his death, this theory remained only unconfirmed and unfinished.
==="Holistic" period (1931–1934)===
{{expand section|date=June 2013}}
The period of major revision of Vygotsky's theory and its transition from mechanist orientation of his 1920s to integrative holistic science of the 1930s. During this period Vygotsky was under particularly strong influence of holistic theories of German-American group of proponents of [[Gestalt psychology]], most notably, the peripheral participants of the Gestalt movement [[Kurt Goldstein]] and [[Kurt Lewin]]. However, Vygotsky's work of this period remained largely fragmentary and unfinished and, therefore, unpublished.


====''Thought and Language''====
==Life and scientific legacy==
Vygotsky was a pioneering psychologist and his major works span six separate volumes, written over roughly ten years, from ''Psychology of Art'' (1925) to ''Thought and Language'' [or ''Thinking and Speech''] (1934). Vygotsky's interests in the fields of [[developmental psychology]], [[child development]], and education were extremely diverse. His philosophical framework includes interpretations of the cognitive role of mediation tools, as well as the re-interpretation of well-known concepts in psychology such as [[internalization]] of knowledge. Vygotsky introduced the notion of [[zone of proximal development]], a metaphor capable of describing the potential of human cognitive development. His work covered topics such as the origin and the [[psychology of art]], development of higher [[mental function]]s, [[philosophy of science]] and the [[Methodology|methodology of psychological research]], the relation between [[learning]] and [[Developmental psychology|human development]], concept formation, interrelation between [[language and thought]] development, play as a psychological phenomenon, [[learning disabilities]], and abnormal human development (aka ''defectology''). His scientific thinking underwent several major transformations throughout his career, but generally Vygotsky's legacy may be divided into two fairly distinct periods,{{Citation needed|date=March 2015}} and the transitional phase between the two during which Vygotsky experienced the crisis in his theory and personal life. These are the [[mechanism (philosophy)|mechanistic]] "instrumental" period of the 1920s, integrative "[[Holism|holistic]]" period of the 1930s, and the transitional years of, roughly, 1929–1931. Each of these periods is characterized by its distinct themes and theoretical innovations.
Perhaps Vygotsky's most important contribution concerns the inter-relationship of language development and thought. This concept, explored in Vygotsky's book ''Thought and Language'', (Russian: ''Myshlenie i rech'', alternative translation: ''Thinking and Speaking'') establishes the explicit and profound connection between speech (both silent inner speech and oral language), and the development of mental concepts and cognitive awareness. Vygotsky described inner speech as being qualitatively different from normal (external) speech. Although Vygotsky believed inner speech developed from external speech via a gradual process of internalization, with younger children only really able to "think out loud," he claimed that in its mature form inner speech would be unintelligible to anyone except the thinker, and would not resemble spoken language as we know it (in particular, being greatly compressed). Hence, thought itself develops socially.


==="Instrumental" period (1920s)===
Language starts as a tool external to the child used for social interaction. The child guides personal behavior by using this tool in a kind of self-talk or "thinking out loud." Initially, self-talk is very much a tool of social interaction and this tapers to negligible levels when the child is alone or with deaf children. Gradually, self-talk is used more as a tool for self-directed and self-regulating behavior. Because speaking has been appropriated and internalized, self-talk is no longer present around the time the child starts school. Self-talk "develops along a rising not a declining, curve; it goes through an evolution, not an involution. In the end, it becomes inner speech" (Vygotsky, 1987, pg 57).


====Cultural mediation and internalization====
Speaking has thus developed along two lines: the line of social communication and the line of inner speech, by which the child mediates and regulates his or her activity through their thoughts. The thoughts, in turn, are mediated by the [[semiotics]] (the meaningful signs) of inner speech. This is not to say that thinking cannot take place without language, but rather that it is mediated by it and thus develops to a much higher level of sophistication. Just as the birthday cake as a sign provides much deeper meaning than its physical properties allow, inner speech as a sign provides much deeper meaning than the lower psychological functions would otherwise allow.
Vygotsky studied child development and the significant roles of [[cultural mediation]] and [[interpersonal communication]]. He observed how higher mental functions developed through these interactions, and also represented the shared knowledge of a culture. This process is known as [[internalization]]. ''Internalization'' may be understood in one respect as "knowing how". For example, the practices of riding a bicycle or pouring a cup of milk, initially, are outside and beyond the child. The mastery of the skills needed for performing these practices occurs through the activity of the child within society. A further aspect of internalization is ''appropriation'', in which children take tools and adapt them to personal use, perhaps using them in unique ways. Internalizing the use of a pencil allows the child to use it very much for personal ends rather than drawing exactly what others in society have drawn previously.


===The period of crisis, criticism, and self-criticism (1929–1932)===
====Zone of proximal development====
"[[Zone of proximal development]]" (ZPD) is Vygotsky’s term for the range of tasks that a child is in the process of learning to complete. The lower limit of ZPD is the level of skill reached by the child working independently (also referred to as the child’s actual developmental level). The upper limit is the level of potential skill that the child is able to reach with the assistance of a more capable instructor.


In the 1930s, Vygotsky was engaged in massive reconstruction of the theory of his "instrumental" period of the 1920s. Around 1929–1930, he realized numerous deficiencies and imperfections of the earlier work of the Vygotsky Circle and criticized it on a number of occasions: in 1929,<ref>Cf. self-criticism of 1929: "I am revising the s[econd] part of "monkey"[i.e., the book Ape, primitive, and child]. Alas! The f[irst] chapter is written wholly according to the Freudianists [...]; then the impenetrable Piaget is turned into an absolute beyond all measure; instrument and sign are mixed together even more, and so on and so forth. This is not the fault of A. R. [Luria] personally, but of the entire "epoch" of our thinking. We need to put a stop to this unrelentingly. [...] Let there be the most rigorous, monastic regime of thought; ideological seclusion, if necessary. And let us demand the same of others. Let us explain that studying cultural psychology is no joke, not something to do at odd moments or among other things, and not grounds for every new person's own conjectures". In: {{cite journal | last1 = Vygotsky | first1 = L. S. | year = 2007 | title = Letters to students and colleagues | journal = Journal of Russian and East European Psychology | volume = 45 | issue = 2| pages = 11–60 | doi = 10.2753/RPO1061-0405450201 | s2cid = 146444813 }}</ref> 1930,<ref>Cf. self-criticism of 1930: "In the process of development, and in the historical development in particular, it is not so much the functions which change (these we mistakenly studies before). Their structure and the system of their development remain the same. What is changed and modified are rather the relationships, the links between the functions. New constellations emerge which were unknown in the preceding stage". In: Vygotsky, L. S. (1930/1997). On psychological systems. In R. W. Rieber & J. Wollock (Eds.), ''The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky'' (Vol. 3. Problems of the Theory and History of Psychology, pp. 91-108). New York: Plenum Press</ref> in 1931,<ref>From the letter to A. R. Luria, from Moscow, June 12, 1931: "I am still beset with thousands of petty chores. The fruitlessness of what I do greatly distresses me. My scientific thinking is going off into the realm of fantasy, and I cannot think things through in a realistic way to the end. Nothing is going right: I am doing the wrong things, writing the wrong things, saying the wrong things. A fundamental reorganization is called for—and this time I am going to carry it out." In: Vygotsky, L. S. (2007). Letters to students and colleagues. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', 45(2), 11-60. {{doi|10.2753/RPO1061-0405450201}}, p. 36</ref> and in 1932.<ref>Vygotsky, archival document of mid-1932 titled "Consciousness without word": "'''Our deficiency is not a deficiency of facts, but the untenability of the theory''': in the analysis of our crisis this is the main difficulty, but not a departure from facts. '''This is ''contra'' A[.]N.[Leontiev.]''' '''Consequently''': salvation is not in the facts but in the theory. We introduced the systemic point of view '''too late'''... '''Now''' I understand all this more deeply" (Zavershneva, 2010b, p. 54)</ref> Specifically, Vygotsky criticized his earlier idea of radical separation between the "lower" and "higher" psychological functions and, around 1932, appears to abandon it.<ref>Vygotsky in his presentation of December 1932, a year and half before his death: "1. The necessity of a new stage of inquiry does not stem from the fact that a new thought has occurred to me or a new idea has caught my interest, but from the necessity of developing the research itself—new facts prod me into searching for new and more intricate explanations. The narrowness, bias, and schematism of the old mindset led us to the wrong assessment of the essential principles that we mistook for the secondary ones: interfunctional connections. We focused attention on the sign (on the tool) to the detriment of the operation with it, representing it as something simple, which goes through three phases: magical—external—internal. But the knot is external and the teenager's diary is external. Hence we have a sea of poorly explained facts and a desire to delve more deeply into the facts, i.e., to evaluate them theoretically in a different way.
Vygotsky viewed the ZPD as a way to better explain the relation between children’s learning and cognitive development. Prior to the ZPD, the relation between learning and development could be boiled down to the following three major positions: 1) Development always precedes learning (e.g., [[Constructivism (learning theory)|constructivism]]): children first need to meet a particular maturation level before learning can occur; 2) Learning and development cannot be separated but instead occur simultaneously (e.g., [[behaviorism]]): essentially, learning is development; and 3) learning and development are separate but interactive processes (e.g., [[gestaltism]]): one process always prepares the other process, and vice versa. Vygotsky rejected these three major theories because he believed that learning always precedes development in the ZPD. In other words, through the assistance of a more capable person, a child is able to learn skills or aspects of a skill that go beyond the child’s actual developmental or maturational level. Therefore, development always follows the child’s potential to learn. In this sense, the ZPD provides a prospective view of cognitive development, as opposed to a retrospective view that characterizes development in terms of a child’s independent capabilities.<ref name="Vygotsky, L. S. 1978">Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Chapter 6 Interaction between learning and development (79-91). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.</ref>
2. The higher and lower functions are not constructed in two tiers: their number and names do not match. But our previous understanding was not right, either [, according to which] a higher function is the mastery of the lower ([e.g.,] voluntary attention is the subordination to it of involuntary attention) because this means exactly—in two tiers". Vygotsky's record titled "Symposium, December 4, 1932", see in Zavershneva, E. 2010b. "The Vygotsky Family Archive: New Findings. Notebooks, Notes, and Scientific Journals of L.S. Vygotsky (1912–1934)". ''Journal of the Russian and East European Psychology'' 48 (1):34-60, pp. 41-42</ref>


Vygotsky's self-criticism was complemented by external criticism for a number of issues, including the separation between the "higher" and the "lower" psychological functions, impracticality and inapplicability of his theory in social practices (such as industry or education) during the time of rapid social change, and vulgar Marxist interpretation of human psychological processes. Critics also pointed to his overemphasis on the role of language and, on the other hand, the ignorance of the emotional factors in human development. Major figures in Soviet psychology such as [[Sergei Rubinstein]] criticized Vygotsky's notion of mediation and its development in the works of students. Following criticism and in response to a generous offer from the highest officials in Soviet Ukraine, a major group of Vygotsky's associates, the members of the [[Vygotsky Circle]], including [[Alexander Luria|Luria]], [[Mark Lebedinsky]], and [[Aleksei N. Leontiev|Leontiev]], moved from Moscow to Ukraine to establish the [[Kharkov School of Psychology|Kharkov school of psychology]]. In the second half of the 1930s, Vygotsky was criticized again for his involvement in the cross-disciplinary study of the child known as [[paedology]] and uncritical borrowings from contemporary "bourgeois" science. Considerable critique came from alleged followers of Vygotsky, such as Leontiev and members of his research group in Kharkov. Much of this early criticism was later discarded by these Vygotskian scholars as well.
[[Instructional scaffolding|Scaffolding]] is a concept closely related to the idea of ZPD, although Vygotsky never actually used the term.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Stone | first1 = C | year = 1998 | title = The metaphor of scaffolding: its utility for the field of learning disabilities | url = | journal = Journal of Learning Disabilities | volume = 31 | issue = 4| pages = 344–364 | doi=10.1177/002221949803100404}}</ref><ref>Verenikina, I. (2003). Understanding scaffolding and the ZPD in educational research. PDF file. Retrieved September 24, 2013, from http://ro.uow.edu.au/edupapers/381/</ref> Scaffolding is changing the level of support to suit the cognitive potential of the child. Over the course of a teaching session, one can adjust the amount of guidance to fit the child’s potential level of performance. More support is offered when a child is having difficulty with a particular task and, over time, less support is provided as the child makes gains on the task. Ideally, scaffolding works to maintain the child’s potential level of development in the ZPD. An essential element to the ZPD and scaffolding is the acquisition of language. According to Vygotsky, language (and in particular, speech) is fundamental to children’s cognitive growth because language provides purpose and intention so that behaviors can be better understood.<ref name="Vygotsky">Vygotsky, L. (1934/1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.</ref> Through the use of speech, children are able to communicate to and learn from others through dialogue, which is an important tool in the ZPD. In a dialogue, a child's unsystematic, disorganized, and spontaneous concepts are met with the more systematic, logical and rational concepts of the skilled helper.<ref name="Santrock">Santrock, J (2004). A Topical Approach To Life-Span Development. Chapter 6 Cognitive Development Approaches (200 – 225). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.</ref> Empirical research suggests that the benefits of scaffolding are not only useful during a task, but can extend beyond the immediate situation in order to influence future cognitive development. For instance, a recent study recorded verbal scaffolding between mothers and their 3- and 4-year-old children as they played together. Then, when the children were six years old, they underwent several measures of [[executive function]], such as working memory and goal-directed play. The study found that the children’s working memory and language skills at six years of age were related to the amount of verbal scaffolding provided by mothers at age three. In particular, scaffolding was most effective when mothers provided explicit conceptual links during play. Therefore, the results of this study not only suggest that verbal scaffolding aids children’s cognitive development, but that the quality of the scaffolding is also important for learning and development.<ref name="Landry">{{cite journal | last1 = Landry | first1 = S. H. | last2 = Miller-Loncar | first2 = C. L. | last3 = Smith | first3 = K. E. | last4 = Swank | first4 = P. R. | year = 2002 | title = The role of early parenting in children's development of executive processes | url = | journal = Developmental Neuropsychology | volume = 21 | issue = | pages = 15–41 | doi=10.1207/s15326942dn2101_2}}</ref>


====Psychology of play====
==="Holistic" period (1932–1934)===
Less known is Vygotsky's research on [[Play (activity)|play]], or children's games, as a psychological phenomenon and its role in the child's development. Through play the child develops abstract meaning separate from the objects in the world, which is a critical feature in the development of higher mental functions. Vygotsky gives the famous example of a child who wants to ride a horse but cannot. If the child were under three, he would perhaps cry and be angry, but around the age of three the child's relationship with the world changes:
{{quote
| Henceforth play is such that the explanation for it must always be that it is the imaginary, illusory realization of unrealizable desires. Imagination is a new formation that is not present in the consciousness of the very raw young child, is totally absent in animals, and represents a specifically human form of conscious activity. Like all functions of consciousness, it originally arises from action.
| Lev Vygotsky
| <ref name="Vygotsky, L. S. 1978"/> }}


There occurred a period of major revision of Vygotsky's theory, a transition from a mechanist orientation of his 1920s to an integrative holistic science of the 1930s. During this period, Vygotsky was under particularly strong influence of holistic theories of German-American group of proponents of [[Gestalt psychology]], most notably, the peripheral participants of the Gestalt movement [[Kurt Goldstein]] and [[Kurt Lewin]]. However, Vygotsky's work of this period remained largely fragmentary and unfinished and, therefore, unpublished.
The child wishes to ride a horse but cannot, so he picks up a stick and stands astride of it, thus pretending he is riding a horse. The stick is a ''pivot''. "Action according to rules begins to be determined by ideas, not by objects.... It is terribly difficult for a child to sever thought (the meaning of a word) from object. Play is a transitional stage in this direction. At that critical moment when a stick – i.e., an object – becomes a pivot for severing the meaning of horse from a real horse, one of the basic psychological structures determining the child’s relationship to reality is radically altered".


====Zone of Proximal Development====
As children get older, their reliance on pivots such as sticks, dolls and other toys diminishes. They have ''internalized'' these pivots as imagination and abstract concepts through which they can understand the world. "The old adage that 'children’s play is imagination in action' can be reversed: we can say that imagination in adolescents and schoolchildren is play without action".<ref name="Vygotsky, L. S. 1978"/>
"[[Zone of Proximal Development]]" (ZPD) is a term Vygotsky used to characterize an individual's mental development. He originally defined the ZPD as “the distance between the actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers.” He used the example of two children in school who originally could solve problems at an eight-year-old developmental level (that is, typical for children who were age 8). After each child received assistance from an adult, one was able to perform at a nine-year-old level and one was able to perform at a twelve-year-old level. He said "This difference between twelve and eight, or between nine and eight, is what we call ''the zone of proximal development.''" He further said that the ZPD “defines those functions that have not yet matured but are in the process of maturation, functions that will mature tomorrow but are currently in an embryonic state.” The zone is bracketed by the learner's current ability and the ability they can achieve with the aid of an instructor of some capacity.<ref>{{Cite web |last=Kurt |first=Dr Serhat |date=October 22, 2022 |title=Vygotsky's Theories and How to Incorporate Vygotsky’s Theories in The Classroom |url=https://educationlibrary.org/vygotskys-theories-and-how-to-incorporate-vygotskys-theories-in-the-classroom/ |access-date=October 23, 2022 |website=Education Library |language=en-US}}</ref>


Vygotsky viewed the ZPD as a better way to explain the relation between children's learning and cognitive development. Prior to the ZPD, the relation between learning and development could be boiled down to the following three major positions: 1) Development always precedes learning (e.g., [[Constructivism (learning theory)|constructivism]]): children first need to meet a particular maturation level before learning can occur; 2) Learning and development cannot be separated, but instead occur simultaneously (e.g., [[behaviorism]]): essentially, learning is development; and 3) learning and development are separate, but interactive processes (e.g., [[gestaltism]]): one process always prepares the other process, and vice versa. Vygotsky rejected these three major theories because he believed that learning should always precede development in the ZPD. According to Vygotsky, through the assistance of a more knowledgeable other, a child is able to learn skills or aspects of a skill that go beyond the child's actual developmental or maturational level. The lower limit of ZPD is the level of skill reached by the child working independently (also referred to as the child's developmental level). The upper limit is the level of potential skill that the child is able to reach with the assistance of a more capable instructor. In this sense, the ZPD provides a prospective view of cognitive development, as opposed to a retrospective view that characterizes development in terms of a child's independent capabilities. The advancement through and attainment of the upper limit of the ZPD is limited by the instructional and scaffolding-related capabilities of the more knowledgeable other (MKO). The MKO is typically assumed to be an older, more experienced teacher or parent, but often can be a learner's peer or someone their junior. The MKO need not even be a person, it can be a machine or book, or other source of visual and/or audio input.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.simplypsychology.org/vygotsky.html|title=Vygotsky &#124; Simply Psychology|date=August 5, 2019|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190805212531/https://www.simplypsychology.org/vygotsky.html|access-date=September 1, 2020|archive-date=August 5, 2019|last1=McLeod |first1=Saul }}</ref>
Vygotsky also referred to the development of social rules that form, for example, when children play house and adopt the roles of different family members. Vygotsky cites an example of two sisters playing being sisters. The rules of behavior between them that go unnoticed in daily life are consciously acquired through play. As well as social rules, the child acquires what we now refer to as [[Self-control|self-regulation]]. For example, when a child stands at the starting line of a running race, she may well desire to run immediately so as to reach the finish line first, but her knowledge of the social rules surrounding the game and her desire to enjoy the game enable her to regulate her initial impulse and wait for the start signal.


====''Thinking and speech''====
==Vygotsky on Education==
Perhaps Vygotsky's most important contribution concerns the inter-relationship of language development and thought. This problem was explored in Vygotsky's book, ''Thinking and speech'', entitled in Russian, ''Myshlenie i rech'' (''Мышление и речь''), that was published in 1934. In fact, this book was a mere collection of essays and scholarly papers that Vygotsky wrote during different periods of his thought development and included writings of his "instrumental" and "holistic" periods. Vygotsky never saw the book published: it was published posthumously, edited by his closest associates (Kolbanovskii, Zankov, and Shif) not sooner than December 1934, i.e., half a year after his death. The first English translation was published in 1962 (with several later revised editions) heavily abbreviated and under an alternative and incorrect translation of the title ''Thought and Language'' for the Russian title ''Mysl' i iazyk''. The book establishes the explicit and profound connection between speech (both silent inner speech and oral language), and the development of mental concepts and cognitive awareness. Vygotsky described inner speech as being qualitatively different from verbal external speech. Although Vygotsky believed inner speech developed from external speech via a gradual process of "internalization" (i.e., transition from the external to the internal), with younger children only really able to "think out loud", he claimed that in its mature form, inner speech would not resemble spoken language as we know it (in particular, being greatly compressed). Hence, thought itself developing socially.


===Death (1934) and posthumous fame===
Vygotsky provides us with a Social Cognitive Learning Theory that we can apply to education. Two critical concepts of his theory include the [[Zone of Proximal Development]] (ZPD), and [[Instructional scaffolding]]. The ZPD is a range of tasks that are within a child’s cognitive ability to learn with assistance. Scaffolding is closely related to this because it is the process of adjusting the amount of support based upon the needs of the child. It was Vygotsky’s intention to suggest that learning occurs in the ZPD where instruction is not too difficult but just exceeds the learner's current independent skills.<ref name="Reid, K.D. 1998">Reid, K.D. (1998). Scaffolding: A broader view. ''Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31''(4), 386-396.</ref> If the material is too difficult, then the child is considered to be in the Frustrational-level of instruction.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=VanDerHeyden|first1=A.M.|title=Using curriculum-based measurement to guide elementary mathematics: Effect on individual and group accountability scores.|journal=Assessment for Effective Intervention|date=2005|volume=30|issue=3|pages=15–31}}</ref> If the material is too easy and the child does not require assistance, the child is considered to be at the Independent-level of learning.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=VanDerHeyden|first1=A.M.|title=Using curriculum-based measurement to guide elementary mathematics: Effect on individual and group accountability scores.|journal=Assessment for Effective Intervention|date=2005|volume=30|issue=3|pages=15–31}}</ref> When learning occurs in the ZPD, it is known as the Instructional-level. [[Curriculum-based measurement]] (CBM) is a criterion-based assessment to generate data about student achievement. Typically CBM is used to measure abilities in mathematics, oral reading fluency, and writing fluency, and to monitor a student's progress throughout the school year.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Steckler|first1=P.M.|last2=Fuchs|first2=L.S.|last3=Fuchs|first3=D.|title=Using curriculum-based measurement to improve student achievement: Review of Research|journal=Psychology in the Schools|date=2005|volume=42|issue=8|pages=795–819}}</ref> A child’s performance across these subjects is compared to literature-based benchmarks that indicate how the child is performing in the current curriculum.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Christ|first1=T.J.|last2=Silberglitt|first2=B.|last3=Yeo|first3=S.|last4=Cormier|first4=D.|title=Curriculum-based measurement of oral reading: An evaluation of growth rates and seasonal effects among students served in general and special education|journal=School Psychology Review|date=2010|volume=39|issue=3|pages=447–462}}</ref> The results of CBM can be used to determine if a gap exists between expected and observed acquisition of skills.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Cusumano|first1=D.L.|title=Is it working?: An overview of curriculum-based measurement and its uses for assessing instructional, intervention, or program effectiveness|journal=The Behavior Analyst|date=2007|volume=8|issue=1|pages=24–34}}</ref>
Vygotsky died of a relapse of [[tuberculosis]] on June 11, 1934, at the age of 37, in Moscow in the [[Soviet Union]]. One of Vygotsky's last private notebook entries gives a proverbial, yet very pessimistic self-assessment of his contribution to psychological theory:


<blockquote>This is the final thing I have done in psychology – and I will like [[Moses]] die at the summit, having glimpsed the promised land but without setting foot on it. Farewell, dear creations. The rest is silence.<ref name="revBook"/></blockquote>
After determining the child’s abilities within the curriculum, determining his/her ZPD, instruction should be provided just above the current ability level. As skills are acquired, the difficulty of the material can be increased, and the process repeats. This process is known as scaffolding. The scaffolding process is particularly useful when engaging children in interactions that would help them to achieve more than they could achieve independently.<ref>Nickel, J. (2011). Early childhood educations students’ emergent skills in literacy scaffolding. ''Canadian Children, 36''(1), 13-19.</ref> One method that a teacher can use to identify the emergence of higher cognitive functioning is by pairing a child with a peer who is more advanced.<ref>Gredler, M. (2012). Understanding Vygotsky for the classroom: Is it too late? ''Educational Psychology Review, 24''(1), 113-131. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-011-9183-6</ref> By doing this, the learner raises himself to a higher level of development through collaboration, to move from the current area of ZPD to the next through peer relation.<ref name="Reid, K.D. 1998"/> Scaffolding through dialogue focuses on the dynamic quality of the personal interaction between teacher and student, and peer and student. This dialogue allows teachers to calibrate instruction for each individual child.<ref name="Reid, K.D. 1998"/>


Immediately after his death, Vygotsky was proclaimed one of the leading psychologists in the Soviet Union, although his stellar reputation was somewhat undermined by the decree of the Central Committee of the Communist Party of 1936 that denounced the mass movement, discipline, and related social practice of the so-called [[Pedology (children study)|pedology]]. Yet, even despite some criticisms and censorship of his works — most notably, in the post-Stalin era by his self-proclaimed best students and followers — Vygotsky always remained among the most quoted scholars in the field and has become a [[Cult following|cult figure]] for a number of contemporary intellectuals and practitioners in Russia and the international psychological and educational community alike.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Fraser | first1 = J. | last2 = Yasnitsky | first2 = A. | year = 2015 | title = Deconstructing Vygotsky's Victimization Narrative: A Re-Examination of the "Stalinist Suppression" of Vygotskian Theory | url = http://individual.utoronto.ca/yasnitsky/texts/Fraser%20&%20Yasnitsky%20(2015).pdf | journal = History of the Human Sciences | volume = 28| issue = 2| pages = 128–153| doi = 10.1177/0952695114560200 | s2cid = 4934828 }}</ref><ref>The last line of the notebook entry, from Shakespeare's ''Hamlet'', 'The rest is silence', was also the last line of Vygotsky's first publication (1915), 'The Tragedy of Hamlet, Prince of Denmark', repr. as ch. 8 in Vygotsky, ''The Psychology of Art'' (1925).</ref>
Teachers recognize the value of peer feedback and deliberately teach peers to give the appropriate feedback. An essential component of cognitive development is socializing with peers via high-quality discussion.<ref>Hattie, J. (2012). ''Visible learning for teachers: Maximizing impact on learning''. New York, NY: Routledge.</ref> For example, each student in a group must learn a specific section of information, which he/she then teaches to the other members of the group.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Walker|first1=I.|last2=Crogan|first2=M.|title=Academic performance, prejudice, and the jigsaw classroom: New pieces to the puzzle.|journal=Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology|date=1998|volume=8|issue=6|pages=381–393}}</ref> A major influence of peer tutoring is that it allows students to become their own teachers. Previous research has shown that peer tutoring has many academic and social benefits for those who are being tutored and those who are tutoring others.<ref name="Routledge">{{cite book|last1=Hattie|first1=J.|title=Visible Learning: A Synthesis of Over 800 Meta-Analyses Relating to Achievement|date=2009|publisher=Routledge|location=New York: NY|pages=186–187}}</ref> In one study, there were strong positive effects on both the students who were being tutored, and the students who were tutors.<ref name="Routledge"/> To summarize, when students collaborate with their peers, learning is maximized. Those who are teaching other students can learn as much as those who are being taught.

==Death==
Vygotsky died of [[tuberculosis]] on June 11, 1934, at the age of 37, in [[Moscow]], [[Soviet Union]].


==Influence worldwide==
==Influence worldwide==


===Eastern Europe===
===Eastern Europe===
In the [[Soviet Union]], the work of the group of Vygotsky's students known as the [[Vygotsky Circle]] was vital for preserving and, in many respects, distorting the scientific legacy of Lev Vygotsky.<ref name="Kozulin 1986 264–274">{{cite journal|last=Kozulin|first=A.|title=The concept of activity in Soviet psychology: Vygotsky, his disciples and critics|journal=American Psychologist|year=1986|volume=41|issue=3|pages=264–274|doi=10.1037/0003-066X.41.3.264}}</ref> The members of the group subsequently laid a foundation for Vygotskian psychology's systematic development in such diverse fields as the psychology of memory ([[Pyotr Zinchenko|P. Zinchenko]]), perception, sensation and movement ([[Alexander Zaporozhets|Zaporozhets]], [[Vladimir Asnin|Asnin]], [[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]]), personality (Lidiya Bozhovich, [[Vladimir Asnin|Asnin]], [[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]]), will and volition ([[Alexander Zaporozhets|Zaporozhets]], [[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]], [[Pyotr Zinchenko|P. Zinchenko]], L. Bozhovich, [[Vladimir Asnin|Asnin]]), psychology of play (G. D. Lukov, Daniil El'konin) and psychology of learning ([[Pyotr Zinchenko|P. Zinchenko]], L. Bozhovich, D. El'konin), as well as the theory of step-by-step formation of mental actions (Pyotr Gal'perin), general psychological [[activity theory]] ([[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]]) and psychology of action ([[Alexander Zaporozhets|Zaporozhets]]).<ref name="Kozulin 1986 264–274"/> Andrey Puzyrey elaborated the ideas of Vygotsky in respect of psychotherapy and even in the broader context of deliberate psychological intervention (psychotechnique), in general.<ref name=Puzyrey>{{cite journal|last=Vassilieva|first=J.|title=Russian psychology at the turn of the 21st century and post-Soviet reforms in the humanities disciplines|journal=History of Psychology|year=2010|volume=13|issue=2|pages=138–159|doi=10.1037/a0019270}}</ref> In Hungary [[Laszlo Garai]] <ref>:Interview with Laszlo Garai on the Activity Theory of Alexis Leontiev and his own Theory of Social Identity as referred to the meta-theory of Lev Vygotsky. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'' 2012; vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 50–64.</ref> founded a Vygotskian research group.
In the [[Soviet Union]], the work of the group of Vygotsky's students known as the [[Vygotsky Circle]] was responsible for Vygotsky's scientific legacy.<ref name="Kozulin 1986 264–274">{{cite journal|last=Kozulin|first=A.|title=The concept of activity in Soviet psychology: Vygotsky, his disciples and critics|journal=American Psychologist|year=1986|volume=41|issue=3|pages=264–274|doi=10.1037/0003-066X.41.3.264}}</ref> The members of the group subsequently laid a foundation for Vygotskian psychology's systematic development in such diverse fields as the psychology of memory ([[Pyotr Zinchenko|P. Zinchenko]]), perception, sensation, and movement ([[Alexander Zaporozhets|Zaporozhets]], [[Vladimir Asnin|Asnin]], [[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]]), personality (Lidiya Bozhovich, [[Vladimir Asnin|Asnin]], [[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]]), will and volition ([[Alexander Zaporozhets|Zaporozhets]], [[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]], [[Pyotr Zinchenko|P. Zinchenko]], L. Bozhovich, [[Vladimir Asnin|Asnin]]), psychology of play (G. D. Lukov, Daniil El'konin) and psychology of learning ([[Pyotr Zinchenko|P. Zinchenko]], L. Bozhovich, D. El'konin), as well as the theory of step-by-step formation of mental actions (Pyotr Gal'perin), general psychological [[activity theory]] ([[Alexei Nikolaevich Leont'ev|A. N. Leont'ev]]) and psychology of action ([[Alexander Zaporozhets|Zaporozhets]]).<ref name="Kozulin 1986 264–274"/> Andrey Puzyrey elaborated the ideas of Vygotsky in respect of psychotherapy and even in the broader context of deliberate [[psychological intervention]] (psychotechnique), in general.<ref name=Puzyrey>{{cite journal|last=Vassilieva|first=J.|title=Russian psychology at the turn of the 21st century and post-Soviet reforms in the humanities disciplines|journal=History of Psychology|year=2010|volume=13|issue=2|pages=138–159|doi=10.1037/a0019270|pmid=20533768}}</ref> [[Laszlo Garai]]<ref>:Interview with Laszlo Garai on the Activity Theory of Alexis Leontiev and his own Theory of Social Identity as referred to the meta-theory of Lev Vygotsky. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'' 2012; vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 50–64.</ref> founded a Vygotskian research group.


===North America===
===North America===
In North America, Vygotsky's work was known from the end of the 1920s through a series of publications in English, but it did not have a major impact on research in general. In 1962 a translation of his posthumous book ''Thinking and Speech'' that came out under the title ''Thought and Language'' did not seem to considerably change the situation. It was only after an eclectic compilation of partly rephrased and partly translated works of Vygotsky and his collaborators that, however, came out under Vygotsky's name in a book titled ''Mind in Society'' that the Vygotsky Boom started in the West: originally, in North America, and later, following the North American example, spread to other regions of the world. This version of Vygotskian science is typically associated with the names of its chief proponents Michael Cole, James Wertsch, their associates and followers, and is relatively well-known under the names of "cultural-historical activity theory" (aka CHAT) or, yet more distant from Vygotsky's legacy, "[[activity theory]]".<ref>{{cite journal|last=Roth|first=W.M.|author2=Lee, J.Y. |title="Vygotsky's Neglected Legacy": Cultural Historical Activity Theory|journal=Review of Educational Research|date=June 2007|volume=77|issue=2|doi=10.3102/0034654306298273}}</ref>
In North America, Vygotsky's work was known from the end of the 1920s through a series of publications in English, but it did not have a major effect on research in general; in fact many scholars have stressed the lack of application to contemporary psychological research.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Vasileva |first=Olga |last2=Balyasnikova |first2=Natalia |date=August 7, 2019 |title=(Re)Introducing Vygotsky’s Thought: From Historical Overview to Contemporary Psychology |url=https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6692430/ |journal=Frontiers in Psychology |volume=10 |pages=1515 |doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01515 |issn=1664-1078 |pmc=6692430 |pmid=31447717}}</ref> In 1962 a translation of his posthumous 1934 book, ''Thinking and Speech'', published with the title,''Thought and Language'', did not seem to change the situation considerably.{{citation needed|date=May 2020}} It was only after an eclectic compilation of partly rephrased and partly translated works of Vygotsky and his collaborators, published in 1978 under Vygotsky's name as ''Mind in Society'', that the Vygotsky boom started in the West: originally, in North America, and later, following the North American example, spread to other regions of the world. A lot of Vigotsky's principles are taught education in today's society.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Marginson |first=Simon |last2=Dang |first2=Thi Kim Anh |date=January 2, 2017 |title=Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory in the context of globalization |url=https://doi.org/10.1080/02188791.2016.1216827 |journal=Asia Pacific Journal of Education |volume=37 |issue=1 |pages=116–129 |doi=10.1080/02188791.2016.1216827 |issn=0218-8791}}</ref>This version of Vygotskian science is typically associated with the names of its chief proponents [[Michael Cole (psychologist)|Michael Cole]], [[James Wertsch]], their associates and followers, and is relatively well known under the names of "[[cultural-historical activity theory]]" (aka CHAT) or "[[activity theory]]".<ref>{{cite journal|last=Roth|first=W.M.|author2=Lee, J.Y. |title="Vygotsky's Neglected Legacy": Cultural Historical Activity Theory|journal=Review of Educational Research|date=June 2007|volume=77|issue=2|doi=10.3102/0034654306298273|pages=186–232|url=http://lchc.ucsd.edu/MCA/Mail/xmcamail.2007_06.dir/att-0021/Wolff-Michael_Roth_Vygotsky.pdf|citeseerx=10.1.1.584.7175|s2cid=12099538}}</ref><ref name="toomela1">{{cite journal | last1 = Toomela | first1 = A | year = 2000 | title = Activity theory is a dead end for cultural-historical psychology | journal = Culture & Psychology | volume = 6 | issue = 3| pages = 353–364 | doi=10.1177/1354067x0063005| s2cid = 143980608 }}</ref><ref name="toomela2">{{cite journal | last1 = Toomela | first1 = A | year = 2008 | title = Activity theory is a dead end for methodological thinking in cultural psychology too | journal = Culture & Psychology | volume = 14 | issue = 3| pages = 289–303 | doi=10.1177/1354067x08088558| s2cid = 144330215 }}</ref> [[Instructional scaffolding|Scaffolding]], a concept introduced by Wood, [[Jerome Bruner|Bruner]], and Ross in 1976, is somewhat related to the idea of ZPD, although Vygotsky never used the term.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Wood | first1 = D. J. | last2 = Bruner | first2 = J. S. | last3 = Ross | first3 = G. | year = 1976 | title = The role of tutoring in problem solving | url = http://isites.harvard.edu/fs/docs/icb.topic850552.files/Wood1976.pdf | journal = Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | volume = 17 | issue = 2| pages = 89–100 | doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x| pmid = 932126 }}</ref>


==Criticisms of North American "Vygotskian" legacy==
==Criticisms of North American "Vygotskian" and original Vygotsky's legacy==
A critique of the North American interpretation of Vygotsky's ideas and, somewhat later, its global spread and dissemination appeared in the 1980s.<ref>Cf. Valsiner, J. (1988). Developmental psychology in the Soviet Union. Brighton, Sussex: Harvester Press, p. 117: Present-day psychologists’ interest in Vygotsky’s thinking is indeed paradoxical. On the one hand, his writings seem increasingly popular among developmental psychologists in Europe and North America. On the other hand, however, careful analyses and thorough understanding of the background of Vygotsky’s ideas is rare... Vygotsky seems to be increasingly well known in international psychology, while remaining little understood. The roots of his thinking in international philosophical and psychological discourse remain largely hidden. His ideas have rarely been developed further, along either theoretical or empirical lines.</ref> The early 1980s criticism of Russian and Western "Vygotskian" scholars<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Simon | first1 = J | year = 1987 | title = Vygotsky and the Vygotskians | url = | journal = American Journal of Education | volume = 95 | issue = 4| pages = 609–613 }}</ref> continued throughout the 1990s. Thus, different authors emphasized the biased and fragmented interpretations of Vygotsky by representatives of what was termed "neo-Vygotskian fashions in contemporary psychology"<ref>Van der Veer, R., and J. Valsiner. 1991. Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. Oxford: Blackwell, p. 1</ref> or "selective traditions" in Vygotskian scholarship.<ref>Cazden, C. B. 1996. Selective traditions: Readings of Vygotsky in writing pedagogy. In Child discourse and social learning: An interdisciplinary perspective, edited by D. Hicks, 165-186. New York: Cambridge University Press</ref> Characteristically, the most fashionable "Vygotskian" phraseology in wide circulation in Western scholarly and educational discourse—such as the so-called "[[zone of proximal development]]"—in the critical literature of this period were referred to as "one of the most used and least understood constructs to appear in contemporary educational literature",<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Palincsar | first1 = A. S. | year = 1998 | title = Keeping the metaphor of scaffolding fresh - a response to C. Addison Stone's "The metaphor of scaffolding: Its utility for the field of learning disabilities | url = | journal = Journal of learning disabilities | volume = 31 | issue = 370-373| page = 370 }}</ref> the construct that was "used as little more than a fashionable alternative to Piagetian terminology or the concept of IQ for describing individual differences in attainment or potential".<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Mercer | first1 = N. | last2 = Fisher | first2 = E. | year = 1992 | title = How do teachers hellp children to learn? An anlysis of teacher's interventions in compter-based activities | url = | journal = Learning and instruction | volume = 2 | issue = 339-355| page = 342 }}</ref> Other authors also suggest clearly distinguishing between original Vygotsky's notion of "zona blizhaishego razvitiia" (''ZBR'') and its later Western superficial interpretations known under the umbrella term "zone of proximal development" (''ZPD'').<ref>Valsiner, J., & Van der Veer, R. (1993). The encoding of distance: The concept of the zone of proximal development and its interpretations. In R. R. Cocking & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), The development and meaning of psychological distance (pp. 35-62). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.</ref> The criticism continued and reached a peak in the 2000s. Most often these critiques address numerous distortions of Vygotsky's ideas, mere "declarations of faith",<ref>Cf. Valsiner, J., and R. Van der Veer (2000). The social mind: Construction of the idea. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, p.4: It is often an open question as to what functions such declarations can have in science. From a position of in-depth analysis, such statements seem merely to be stating the obvious (compared with the statements like the rain is wet or the rich are affluent). And yet, such general claims about the sociality of the human psyche are made with remarkable vigour and repetitiveness</ref> "versions of Vygotsky",<ref>Gillen, J. (2000). Versions of Vygotsky. British Journal of Educational Studies 48 (2):183—98</ref> the "concepts and inferences curiously attributed to Lev Vygotsky",<ref>Gredler, M. E. 2007. Of cabbages and kings: Concepts and inferences curiously attributed to Lev Vygotsky (Commentary on McVee, Dunsmore, and Gavelek, 2005). Review of Educational Research 77 (2):233-238</ref> "multiple readings of Vygotsky",<ref>van der Veer, R. 2008. Multiple readings of Vygotsky. In The transformation of learning: Advances in cultural-historical activity theory, edited by B. van Oers, W. Wardekker, E. Elbers and R. van der Veer, 20-37. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press</ref> some of which—for instance, "activity theory"—are referred to as "dead end” for cultural-historical psychology<ref>Toomela, A. 2000. Activity theory is a dead end for cultural-historical psychology. Culture & Psychology 6 (3):353-364</ref> and, moreover, for methodological thinking in cultural psychology.<ref>Toomela, A. 2008. Activity theory is a dead end for methodological thinking in cultural psychology too. Culture & Psychology 14 (3):289-303</ref> Some publications question "if anyone actually reads Vygotsky’s words",<ref>Gredler, M. E., and C. S. Schields. 2004. Does no one read Vygotsky's words? Commentary on Glassman. Educational Researcher 33 (2):21-25</ref> whether it is "too late to understand Vygotsky for the classroom",<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gredler | first1 = M.E. | year = 2012 | title = Understanding Vygotsky for the classroom: Is it too late? | url = | journal = [[Educational Psychology Review]] | volume = 24 | issue = 1| pages = 113–131 | doi=10.1007/s10648-011-9183-6}}</ref> and suggest "turning Vygotsky on his head."<ref>Rowlands, S. [http://link.springer.com/article/10.1023%2FA%3A1008748901374?LI=true Turning Vygotsky on His Head: Vygotsky's "Scientifically Based Method" and the Socioculturalist's "Social Other"]. Science & Education, vol. 9, Issue 6, p.537-575</ref> Inconsistencies, contradictions, and at times fundamental flaws in "Vygotskian" literature were revealed in the ocean of critical publications on this subject and are typically associated with—but certainly not limited to—the North American legacy of Michael Cole and James Wertsch and their associates.<ref>For massive criticism of these two particular research traditions see Miller, R. (2011). Vygotsky in perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.</ref> These criticisms contributed significantly to the increasing awareness of numerous "challenges of claiming a Vygotskian perspective".<ref>Smagorinsky, P. 2011. [http://www.springerlink.com/content/m746w616382651uh/ Vygotsky and Literacy Research: A Methodological Framework]. Rotterdam & Boston: Sense.</ref>
A critique of the North American interpretation of Vygotsky's ideas and, somewhat later, its global spread and dissemination appeared in the 1980s.<ref>Cf. Valsiner, J. (1988). Developmental psychology in the Soviet Union. Brighton, Sussex: Harvester Press, p. 117: Present-day psychologists’ interest in Vygotsky's thinking is indeed paradoxical. On the one hand, his writings seem increasingly popular among developmental psychologists in Europe and North America. On the other hand, however, careful analyses and thorough understanding of the background of Vygotsky's ideas is rare... Vygotsky seems to be increasingly well known in international psychology, while remaining little understood. The roots of his thinking in international philosophical and psychological discourse remain largely hidden. His ideas have rarely been developed further, along either theoretical or empirical lines.</ref> The early 1980s criticism of Russian and Western "Vygotskian" scholars <ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Simon | first1 = J | year = 1987 | title = Vygotsky and the Vygotskians | journal = American Journal of Education | volume = 95 | issue = 4| pages = 609–613 | doi = 10.1086/444328 | s2cid = 142282282 }}</ref> continued throughout the 1990s. Van der Veer & Valsiner (1991) called these strands of Vygotskian thought "neo-Vygotskian fashions in contemporary psychology"<ref>Van der Veer, R., and J. Valsiner. 1991. Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. Oxford: Blackwell, p. 1</ref> and Cazden (1996) called them "selective traditions" in Vygotskian scholarship.<ref>Cazden, C. B. 1996. Selective traditions: Readings of Vygotsky in writing pedagogy. In Child discourse and social learning: An interdisciplinary perspective, edited by D. Hicks, 165-186. New York: Cambridge University Press</ref> Palincsar (1998) said that the zone of proximal development was "one of the most used and least understood constructs to appear in contemporary educational literature",<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Palincsar | first1 = A. S. | year = 1998 | title = Keeping the metaphor of scaffolding fresh - a response to C. Addison Stone's "The metaphor of scaffolding: Its utility for the field of learning disabilities | url =https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/68637/2/10.1177_002221949803100406.pdf | journal = Journal of Learning Disabilities | volume = 31 | issue = 4 | pages = 370–373| doi = 10.1177/002221949803100406 | pmid = 9666613 | hdl = 2027.42/68637 | s2cid = 26881323 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> and Fischer & Mercer (1992) said that the construct was "used as little more than a fashionable alternative to Piagetian terminology or the concept of IQ for describing individual differences in attainment or potential".<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Mercer | first1 = N. | last2 = Fisher | first2 = E. | year = 1992 | title = How do teachers help children to learn? An analysis of teacher's interventions in compter-based activities | journal = Learning and Instruction | volume = 2 | issue = 339–355| page = 342 | doi = 10.1016/0959-4752(92)90022-E }}</ref>


Van der Veer and Valsiner also suggest clearly distinguishing between Vygotsky's original notion of "zona blizhaishego razvitiia" (''ZBR'') and what they think of as its "superficial interpretations" collectively known as "zone of proximal development" (''ZPD'').<ref>Valsiner, J., & Van der Veer, R. (1993). The encoding of distance: The concept of the zone of proximal development and its interpretations. In R. R. Cocking & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), The development and meaning of psychological distance (pp. 35-62). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.</ref><ref>Valsiner, J., & van der Veer, R. (2014). Encountering the border: Vygotsky's zona blizaishego razvitya and its implications for theory of development. In A. Yasnitsky, R. van der Veer, & M. Ferrari (Eds.), ''The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology''. (pp. 148-174). Cambridge University Press.</ref> Valsiner and Van der Veer also identify certain statements in Vygotskian literature as mere "declarations of faith", i.e. hollow statements often repeated.<ref>Cf. Valsiner, J., and R. Van der Veer (2000). The social mind: Construction of the idea. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, p.4: It is often an open question as to what functions such declarations can have in science. From a position of in-depth analysis, such statements seem merely to be stating the obvious (compared with the statements like the rain is wet or the rich are affluent). And yet, such general claims about the sociality of the human psyche are made with remarkable vigour and repetitiveness</ref> Whereas Gillen (2000) talks of different "versions of Vygotsky"<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gillen | first1 = J | year = 2000 | title = Versions of Vygotsky | journal = British Journal of Educational Studies | volume = 48 | issue = 2| pages = 183–98 | doi=10.1111/1467-8527.t01-1-00141| s2cid = 8639392 }}</ref> and Van der Veer (2008) speaks of "multiple readings of Vygotsky",<ref>van der Veer, R. 2008. Multiple readings of Vygotsky. In The transformation of learning: Advances in cultural-historical activity theory, edited by B. van Oers, W. Wardekker, E. Elbers and R. van der Veer, 20-37. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press</ref> Gradler (2007) simply speaks of "concepts and inferences curiously attributed to Lev Vygotsky".<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gredler | first1 = M. E. | year = 2007 | title = Of cabbages and kings: Concepts and inferences curiously attributed to Lev Vygotsky (Commentary on McVee, Dunsmore, and Gavelek, 2005) | journal = Review of Educational Research | volume = 77 | issue = 2| pages = 233–238 | doi=10.3102/0034654306298270| s2cid = 145115751 }}</ref> Toomela (2000) calls the strand known as "activity theory" a "dead end” for cultural-historical psychology <ref name="toomela1"/> and, moreover, for methodological thinking in cultural psychology.<ref name="toomela2"/>
==Criticisms of available Vygotsky's texts==
A relatively recent trend in Vygotskian science emerged in the 1990s. This trend is typically associated with growing dissatisfaction with the quality and scholarly integrity of available English translations of the texts of Vygotsky and members of [[Vygotsky Circle]] made from largely mistaken, distorted, and even in a few instances falsified Soviet editions,<ref>Veer, R., van der (1997). Translator's foreword and acknowledgments. In: Rieber, R.W. & Wollock, J. (Eds.), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Vol.3. Problems of the theory and history of psychology, pp. v-vi. New York-London: Plenum Press</ref><ref>van der Veer, R. (1998). Book review: L. S. Vygotsky. Educational Psychology. Robert Silverman, Trans. Boca Raton FL: St. Lucie Press, 1997. 374 pp. $39.95. ISBN 1-878205-15-3. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences: Vol. 34(4), 430–431</ref> which raises serious concerns about the reliability of Vygotsky's texts available in English.<ref name="html">van der Veer, R. & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.springerlink.com/content/278j5025767m2263/ Vygotsky in English: What Still Needs to Be Done]. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science [http://www.springerlink.com/content/278j5025767m2263/fulltext.html html], [http://www.springerlink.com/content/278j5025767m2263/fulltext.pdf pdf]</ref> However, unlike critical literature that discusses Western interpretations of Vygotsky's legacy, the target of criticism and the primary object of research in the studies of the revisionist strand are Vygotsky's texts proper: the manuscripts, original lifetime publications, and Vygotsky's posthumous Soviet editions that most often were subsequently uncritically translated into other languages. The revisionist strand is solidly grounded in a series of studies in Vygotsky's archives that uncovered the previously unknown and unpublished Vygotsky's materials.<ref name="Zavershneva, E 2009"/><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2007. "Put' k svobode" (K publikatsii materialov iz semejnogo arkhiva L.S. Vygotskogo) ["The road to freedom" (To the publication of the materials from the family archive of L.S. Vygotsky)]. Novoe literaturnoe obozrenie 85 (5):67-90</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2008a. Zapisnye knizhki, zametki, nauchnye dnevniki L.S. Vygotskogo: rezul'taty issledovaniya semejnogo arkhiva [Notebooks, notes, scientific diaries of L.S. Vygotsky: the results of the investigation of the family archive, part 1]. Voprosy psikhologii (1):132—145.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2008b. Zapisnye knizhki, zametki, nauchnye dnevniki L.S. Vygotskogo: rezul'taty issledovaniya semejnogo arkhiva [Notebooks, notes, scientific diaries of L.S. Vygotsky: the results of the investigation of the family archive, part 2]. Voprosy psikhologii (2):120-136.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2010a. The Vygotsky Family Archive (1912–1934). New Findings. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology 48 (1):14-33.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2010b. The Vygotsky Family Archive: New Findings. Notebooks, Notes, and Scientific Journals of L.S. Vygotsky (1912–1934). Journal of Russian and East European Psychology 48 (1):34-60.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2010c. "The Way to Freedom" (On the Publication of Documents from the Family Archive of Lev Vygotsky). Journal of Russian and East European Psychology 48 (1):61-90.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2012a. Evreiskii vopros v neopublikovannykh rukopisiakh L.S. Vygotskogo [Jewish question in the unpublished manuscripts of L.S. Vygotsky]. Voprosy psikhologii (2):79-99.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2012. "The Key to Human Psychology". Commentary on L.S. Vygotsky’s Notebook from the Zakharino Hospital (1926). Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012</ref>


Gredler & Schields (2004) question "if anyone actually reads Vygotsky's words"<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gredler | first1 = M. E. | last2 = Schields | first2 = C. S. | year = 2004 | title = Does no one read Vygotsky's words? Commentary on Glassman | journal = Educational Researcher | volume = 33 | issue = 2| pages = 21–25 | doi=10.3102/0013189x033002021| s2cid = 145203451 }}</ref> and Gredler (2012) asks whether it is "too late to understand Vygotsky for the classroom",<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gredler | first1 = M.E. | year = 2012 | title = Understanding Vygotsky for the classroom: Is it too late? | journal = [[Educational Psychology Review]] | volume = 24 | issue = 1| pages = 113–131 | doi=10.1007/s10648-011-9183-6| s2cid = 144839041 }}</ref> while Rowlands (2000) suggests "turning Vygotsky on his head".<ref>Rowlands, S. [https://link.springer.com/article/10.1023%2FA%3A1008748901374?LI=true Turning Vygotsky on His Head: Vygotsky's "Scientifically Based Method" and the Socioculturalist's "Social Other"]. Science & Education, vol. 9, Issue 6, p.537-575</ref> According to Miller (2011), inconsistencies, contradictions, and at times fundamental flaws in "Vygotskian" literature were revealed in critical publications on this subject and are typically associated with - but certainly not limited to - the North American legacy of Michael Cole and James Wertsch and their associates.<ref>Miller, R. (2011). Vygotsky in perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.</ref> Smagorinsky (2011) speaks of "challenges of claiming a Vygotskian perspective".<ref>Smagorinsky, P. 2011. [https://doi.org/10.1007%2F978-94-6091-696-0_1 Vygotsky and Literacy Research: A Methodological Framework]. Rotterdam & Boston: Sense.</ref>
Thus, some studies of the revisionist strand show that certain phrases, terms, and expressions typically associated with Vygotskian legacy as its core notions and concepts—such as "cultural-historical psychology", "cultural-historical theory", "cultural-historical school", "higher psychical/mental functions", "internalization", "zone of proximal development", etc., -- in fact, either occupy not more than just a few dozen pages within the six-volume collection of Vygotsky’s works<ref>Tudge, J. 1999. Discovering Vygotsky: A Historical and Developmental Approach to His Theory. In Undiscovered Vygotsky. Etudes on the Pre-history of Cultural-Historical Psychology, ed. N. Veresov, pp. 10–17. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.</ref><ref>Chaiklin, Seth. 2003. The Zone of Proximal Development in Vygotsky’s Analysis of Learning and Instruction. In Vygotsky’s Educational Theory in Cultural Context, ed. A. Kozulin, V.S. Ageyev, S.M. Miller, and B. Gindis, pp. 39–64. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.</ref> or even never occur in Vygotsky's own writings.<ref>Keiler, P. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/2012n1a1/2012n1a1.1.pdf «Cultural-Historical Theory» and «Cultural-Historical School»: From Myth (Back) to Reality] // [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1)], 1—33</ref> Another series of studies revealed the questionable quality of Vygotsky's published texts that, in fact, were never finished and intended for publication by their author,<ref name="ReferenceA"/><ref name="ReferenceB"/><ref>Ясницкий, А. (2011). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/2011n4a1/2011n4a1.pdf «Когда б вы знали, из какого сора...»: К определению состава и хронологии создания основных работ Выготского] ["I Wish You Knew From What Stray Matter...": Identifying the set of Vygotsky's major oeuvre and determining the chronology of their composition]. [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/index.php 4(4)], 1-52</ref> but were nevertheless posthumously published without giving proper editorial acknowledgement of their unfinished, transitory nature<ref name="ReferenceC"/><ref>E. Iu. Zavershneva and M.E. Osipov. Primary Changes to the Version of "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology" Published in the Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012</ref> and with numerous editorial interventions and distortions of Vygotsky's text.<ref>Tulviste, P. 1987. Shestitomnoe izdanie trudov L.S. Vygotskogo [Six-volume edition of L.S. Vygotsky's oeuvre]. Voprosy psikhologii, no. 2, pp. 170–73.</ref><ref>Mecacci, L. 1990. "Edizioni e traduzioni di Pensiero e linguaggio." In Vygotskij, L.S. Pensiero e Linguaggio. Ricerche psicologiche, pp. xv–xviii. Roma: Laterza.</ref><ref>Brushlinskii, A. V. (1996). Pervye utochneniya tekstov L.S. Vygotskogo [First clarifications of L.S. Vygotsky's published texts]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 17, 19–25</ref><ref>Peshkov, I. V. (1999). Tekstologicheskij kommentarij [Textological commentary]. In L. S. Vygotskii, Thinking and speech’ (pp. 339). Moscow: Labirint.</ref><ref>Peshkov, I. V. (2008). Tsenzura stilya ne rekomenduetsya [Style censorship is not recommended]. In L. S. Vygotskii (Ed.), Psikhologiya iskusstva (pp. 338–340). Moscow: Labirint</ref><ref>Kellogg, D. & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/2011n4a4/2011n4a4.pdf The differences between the Russian and English texts of Tool and Symbol in Child Development. Supplementary and analytic materials]. [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/index.php 4(4)], 98-158</ref><ref>Mecacci, L., & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/2011n4a5/2011n4a5.pdf Editorial Changes in the Three Russian Editions of Vygotsky's Thinking and Speech (1934, 1956, 1982): Towards Authoritative and Ultimate English Translation of the Book]. [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/index.php 4(4)], 159-187</ref> Another series of publications reveals that another well-known Vygotsky's text that is often presented as the foundational work was back-translated into Russian from an English translation of a lost original and passed for the original Vygotsky's writing. This episode was referred to as "benign forgery".<ref>[[Elkhonon Goldberg|Goldberg E.]] [http://books.google.ca/books?id=ldWTnKHfJjkC&pg=PA99&lpg=PA99&dq=%22benign+forgery The wisdom paradox: How your mind can grow stronger as your brain grows older]. New York: Gotham, 2005, p. 99</ref><ref>Rieber, R., & Robinson, D. (2004). [http://books.google.ca/books?id=uGp6U1WqFi4C&pg=PR16&lpg=PR16&dq=%22The+Russian+original+had+apparently+been+lost%22 Preface. In R. W. Rieber & D. K. Robinson (Eds.), The essential Vygotsky] (pp. xiii-xvii). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.</ref><ref>Goldberg, Е. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.3.1.pdf "Thank you for sharing this fascinating material - very interesting"] // [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1)], 118-120</ref><ref>Cole, M. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.4.1.pdf Comments on prior Comments] // [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1)], 124-127</ref><ref>van der Veer, R. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.5.1.pdf Rukopisi ne goryat or do they?] // [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1), 133-138]</ref>


Lambert (2000) claims that Vygotsky's writings on play were too brief to be called a theory and furthermore were hardly relevant anymore.<ref>{{cite journal |first=E. Beverley |last=Lambert |year=2000 |title=Questioning Vygotsky's 'Theory' of Play |journal=Early Child Development and Care |volume=160 |issue=1 |pages=25–31 |doi=10.1080/0030443001600103|s2cid=144872498 }}</ref> According to Zhang (2013), Vygotsky's theories are fundamentally flawed from a contemporary linguistic standpoint<ref name="zhang2013">Zhang, R. [張芮菡]. (2013). [http://hub.hku.hk/handle/10722/197513 Rethinking Vygotsky : a critical reading of Vygotsky's cultural-historical theory and its appropriation in contemporary scholarship]. (Thesis). University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong SAR. Retrieved from [https://dx.doi.org/10.5353/th_b5194765dx.doi.org]</ref><ref name=zhang2018>Zhang, R. (2018). [https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9781351060622/chapters/10.4324%2F9781351060639-3 Rethinking Vygotsky: A Critical reading of the semiotics in Vygotsky's cultural-historical theory]. In Yasnitsky, A. (Ed.) [https://www.routledge.com/Questioning-Vygotskys-Legacy-Scientific-Psychology-or-Heroic-Cult/Yasnitsky/p/book/9781138481268 Questioning Vygotsky's Legacy: Scientific Psychology or Heroic Cult]. New York & London: Routledge</ref> and Newman (2018) claims that "the support claimed from Vygotsky in accounts of second language acquisition is misplaced, first because of those difficulties and, second, because many who claim support from Vygotsky, do not need or even use his theory but instead focus their attention on his empirical observations and assume incorrectly that if their own empirical observations match Vygotsky's, then Vygotsky's theory can be accepted".<ref name="newman">Newman, S. (2018). [https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jtsb.12174 Vygotsky, Wittgenstein, and sociocultural theory]. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 48(3), 350-368</ref>
==''Complete Works of L.S. Vygotsky''==

Scholars associated with the revisionist strand in Vygotskian science propose returning to Vygotsky's original uncensored works, critically revising the available discourse, and republishing them in both Russian and translation with a rigorous scholarly commentary.<ref name="html"/><ref>Veer, R., van der (2002). Vygotsky in English: What still needs to be done. Website for International Cultural Historical Studies (http://www.ichs.udk.-berlin.de)</ref> Therefore, an essential part of this revisionist strand is the ongoing work on "''PsyAnima'' Complete Vygotsky" project<ref>[http://psyanimajournal.livejournal.com/ psyanimajournal]: [http://psyanimajournal.livejournal.com/3526.html PsyAnima Полное собрание сочинений Выготского / PsyAnima Complete Vygotsky]</ref> that for the first time ever exposes full collections of Vygotsky's texts, uncensored and cleared from numerous mistakes, omissions, insertions, and blatant distortions and falsifications of the author's text made in Soviet editions and uncritically transferred in virtually all foreign translated editions of Vygotsky's works. This project is affiliated with [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]] and is carried out by a number of members of the journal's editorial board in collaboration with an international team of enthusiasts—researchers, archival workers, and library staff—from Belarus, Brazil, Canada, Israel, Italy, the Netherlands, Russia, and Switzerland, who joined their efforts and put together a collection of L.S. Vygotsky’s texts. This publication work is supported by a stream of critical scholarly studies and publications on textology, history, theory and methodology of Vygotskian research that cumulatively contributes to the first ever edition of ''The Complete Works of L.S. Vygotsky''.<ref>Yasnitsky, A. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2012/3/2012n3a6/2012n3a6.2.pdf The Complete Works of L.S. Vygotsky: ''PsyAnima Complete Vygotsky'' project]. PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 5(3), 144-148</ref>
==Revisionist movement in Vygotsky Studies==
During the early twenty-first century, several scholarly reevaluations of the popular version (sometimes disparagingly termed "Vygotsky cult", "the cult of Vygotsky", or even "the cult of personality around Vygotsky") of Vygotsky's legacy have been undertaken and are referred to as the "revisionist revolution in Vygotsky Studies".<ref name="revBook">Yasnitsky, A. & van der Veer, R. (Eds.) (2015). [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/278037138_Revisionist_Revolution_in_Vygotsky_Studies Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies]. London and New York: Routledge</ref><ref name="vygRevisitado">Yasnitsky, A., van der Veer, R., Aguilar, E. & García, L.N. (Eds.) (2016). [http://www.minoydavila.com/vygotski-revisitado-una-historia-critica-de-su-contexto-y-legado.html Vygotski revisitado: una historia crítica de su contexto y legado] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180817023033/http://www.minoydavila.com/vygotski-revisitado-una-historia-critica-de-su-contexto-y-legado.html |date=August 17, 2018 }}. Buenos Aires: Miño y Dávila Editores</ref><ref name="maidan">{{cite journal | last1 = Maidansky | first1 = A | year = 2020| title = Revisionist revolution in Vygotsky studies | journal = Studies in East European Thought | volume = 72 | pages = 89–95 | doi = 10.1007/s11212-020-09359-1 | s2cid = 216343144 | url = http://dspace.bsu.edu.ru/bitstream/123456789/38195/1/Maidansky_Anton_Yasnitsky.pdf }}</ref> Vygotsky studies conducted within the framework of the "revisionist turn" during the 2010s revealed systematic and massive falsifications and distortions of Vygotsky's legacy., but also demonstrated a rapid and dramatic decrease of this author's popularity in international scholarship that began in 2017.{{citation needed|date=May 2020}} The reasons of this crisis are not entirely clear yet and are being discussed in scholarly circles.<ref name="vygScienceSuperman"/><ref name="vygLeftistBolshevik">Yasnitsky, A. (2018). [http://individual.utoronto.ca/yasnitsky/texts/VygotskyBolshevikCritique.pdf Vygotsky's Marxism: A 21st Century Leftist Bolshevik Critique] («Le marxisme de Vygotski: Le 21e siecle critique gauchiste bolchevique»). Discussion paper presented on June 22, 2018, at the [https://www.unige.ch/SIV2018/concept/ 7e Seminaire international Vygotski] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180911114850/https://www.unige.ch/SIV2018/concept/ |date=September 11, 2018 }} held at the Universite de Geneve, June 20–22, 2018, Geneva, Switzerland.</ref>

The revisionist movement in Vygotsky Studies was termed a "revisionist revolution"<ref name="revBook"/> to describe a relatively recent trend that emerged in the 1990s. This trend is typically associated with growing dissatisfaction with the quality and scholarly integrity of available texts of Vygotsky and members of [[Vygotsky Circle]], including their English translations made from largely mistaken, distorted, and even in a few instances falsified Soviet editions,<ref>Veer, R., van der (1997). Translator's foreword and acknowledgments. In: Rieber, R.W. & Wollock, J. (Eds.), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Vol.3. Problems of the theory and history of psychology, pp. v-vi. New York-London: Plenum Press</ref><ref>van der Veer, R. (1998). Book review: L. S. Vygotsky. Educational Psychology. Robert Silverman, Trans. Boca Raton FL: St. Lucie Press, 1997. 374 pp. $39.95. {{ISBN|1-878205-15-3}}. ''Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences'' Vol. 34(4), 430–431</ref> which raises serious concerns about the reliability of Vygotsky's texts available in English.<ref name="html">{{cite journal | last1 = van der Veer | first1 = R. | last2 = Yasnitsky | first2 = A. | year = 2011 | title = Vygotsky in English: What Still Needs to Be Done | journal = Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science | volume = 45| issue = 4| pages = 475–493 | doi = 10.1007/s12124-011-9172-9 | pmid = 21626141 | pmc = 3181411 }}</ref> However, unlike critical literature that discusses Western interpretations of Vygotsky's legacy, the target of criticism and the primary object of research in the studies of the revisionist strand are Vygotsky's texts proper: the manuscripts, original lifetime publications, and Vygotsky's posthumous Soviet editions that most often were subsequently uncritically translated into other languages. The revisionist strand is solidly grounded in a series of studies in Vygotsky's archives that uncovered previously unknown and unpublished Vygotsky materials.<ref name="Zavershneva, E 2009"/><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2007. "Put' k svobode" (K publikatsii materialov iz semejnogo arkhiva L.S. Vygotskogo) ["The road to freedom" (To the publication of the materials from the family archive of L.S. Vygotsky)]. Novoe literaturnoe obozrenie 85 (5):67-90</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2008a. Zapisnye knizhki, zametki, nauchnye dnevniki L.S. Vygotskogo: rezul'taty issledovaniya semejnogo arkhiva [Notebooks, notes, scientific diaries of L.S. Vygotsky: the results of the investigation of the family archive, part 1]. Voprosy psikhologii (1):132—145.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2008b. Zapisnye knizhki, zametki, nauchnye dnevniki L.S. Vygotskogo: rezul'taty issledovaniya semejnogo arkhiva [Notebooks, notes, scientific diaries of L.S. Vygotsky: the results of the investigation of the family archive, part 2]. Voprosy psikhologii (2):120-136.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Zavershneva | first1 = E | year = 2010a | title = The Vygotsky Family Archive (1912–1934). New Findings | journal = Journal of Russian and East European Psychology | volume = 48 | issue = 1| pages = 14–33 | doi=10.2753/rpo1061-0405480101| s2cid = 143118053 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Zavershneva | first1 = E | year = 2010b | title = The Vygotsky Family Archive: New Findings. Notebooks, Notes, and Scientific Journals of L.S. Vygotsky (1912–1934) | journal = Journal of Russian and East European Psychology | volume = 48 | issue = 1| pages = 34–60 | doi=10.2753/rpo1061-0405480102| s2cid = 142733451 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Zavershneva | first1 = E | year = 2010c | title = "The Way to Freedom" (On the Publication of Documents from the Family Archive of Lev Vygotsky) | journal = Journal of Russian and East European Psychology | volume = 48 | issue = 1| pages = 61–90 | doi=10.2753/rpo1061-0405480103| s2cid = 149007217 }}</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2012a. Evreiskii vopros v neopublikovannykh rukopisiakh L.S. Vygotskogo [Jewish question in the unpublished manuscripts of L.S. Vygotsky]. Voprosy psikhologii (2):79-99.</ref><ref>Zavershneva, E. 2012. "The Key to Human Psychology". Commentary on L.S. Vygotsky's Notebook from the Zakharino Hospital (1926)" ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012</ref>

Thus, some studies of the revisionist strand show that certain phrases, terms, and expressions typically associated with Vygotskian legacy as its core notions and concepts—such as "cultural-historical psychology",<ref name="handbook">Yasnitsky, A., van der Veer, R., & Ferrari, M. (Eds.) (2014). [http://www.cambridge.org/ca/academic/subjects/psychology/developmental-psychology/cambridge-handbook-cultural-historical-psychology The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press</ref><ref>Keiler, P. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a1/2012n1a1.1.pdf «Cultural-Historical Theory» and «Cultural-Historical School»: From Myth (Back) to Reality] // PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1), 1—33] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130204083839/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php |date=February 4, 2013 }}</ref><ref>Кайлер, П. [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a1/2012n1a1.2.pdf «Культурно-историческая теория» и «культурно-историческая школа»: От мифа (обратно) к реальности] // Психологический журнал Международного университета природы, общества и человека «Дубна», [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php ibid., с. 34—46] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130204083839/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php |date=February 4, 2013 }} (in Russian)</ref><ref>Keiler, P. (2018). A history of the social construction of the “cultural-historical”. In Yasnitsky, A. (Ed.) [https://www.routledge.com/Questioning-Vygotskys-Legacy-Scientific-Psychology-or-Heroic-Cult/Yasnitsky/p/book/9781138481268 Questioning Vygotsky's Legacy: Scientific Psychology or Heroic Cult]. New York & London: Routledge</ref> "cultural-historical theory", "cultural-historical school", "higher psychical/mental functions", "internalization", "zone of proximal development", etc. in fact, either occupy not more than just a few dozen pages within the six-volume collection of Vygotsky's works,<ref>Tudge, J. 1999. Discovering Vygotsky: A Historical and Developmental Approach to His Theory. In Undiscovered Vygotsky. Etudes on the Pre-history of Cultural-Historical Psychology, ed. N. Veresov, pp. 10–17. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.</ref><ref>Chaiklin, Seth. 2003. The Zone of Proximal Development in Vygotsky's Analysis of Learning and Instruction. In Vygotsky's Educational Theory in Cultural Context, ed. A. Kozulin, V.S. Ageyev, S.M. Miller, and B. Gindis, pp. 39–64. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.</ref> or never even occur in Vygotsky's own writings.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Keiler | first1 = P | year = 2012 | title = "Cultural-Historical Theory" and "Cultural-Historical School": From Myth (Back) to Reality | url = http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a1/2012n1a1.1.pdf | journal = [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]] | volume = 5 | issue = 1 | pages = 1–33 | url-status = dead | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20130921060750/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a1/2012n1a1.1.pdf | archive-date = September 21, 2013 }}</ref> Another series of studies revealed the questionable quality of Vygotsky's published texts that, in fact, were never finished and intended for publication by their author,<ref name="ReferenceA"/><ref name="ReferenceB"/><ref>{{cite journal |last=Ясницкий |first=А. |year=2011 |url=http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/2011n4a1/2011n4a1.pdf |title="Когда б вы знали, из какого сора...": К определению состава и хронологии создания основных работ Выготского |language=ru |trans-title=“When only you knew what rubbish ...”: On defining the composition and chronology of the creation of Vygotsky's main works |issn=2076-7099 |issue=4 |pages=1–52 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131102042014/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/2011n4a1/2011n4a1.pdf |archive-date=November 2, 2013 |journal=[[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]]}}<br />[http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php ''PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal'' (4)] {{webarchive |url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131220025809/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php |date=December 20, 2013 }}</ref> but were nevertheless posthumously published without giving proper editorial acknowledgement of their unfinished, transitory nature,<ref name="ReferenceC"/><ref>E. Iu. Zavershneva and M.E. Osipov. Primary Changes to the Version of "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology" Published in the Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. ''Journal of Russian and East European Psychology'', vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012</ref> and with numerous editorial interventions and distortions of Vygotsky's text.<ref>Tulviste, P. 1987. Shestitomnoe izdanie trudov L.S. Vygotskogo [Six-volume edition of L.S. Vygotsky's oeuvre]. Voprosy psikhologii, no. 2, pp. 170–73.</ref><ref>Mecacci, L. 1990. "Edizioni e traduzioni di Pensiero e linguaggio." In Vygotskij, L.S. Pensiero e Linguaggio. Ricerche psicologiche, pp. xv–xviii. Roma: Laterza.</ref><ref>Brushlinskii, A. V. (1996). Pervye utochneniya tekstov L.S. Vygotskogo [First clarifications of L.S. Vygotsky's published texts]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 17, 19–25</ref><ref>Peshkov, I. V. (1999). Tekstologicheskij kommentarij [Textological commentary]. In L. S. Vygotskii, Thinking and speech’ (pp. 339). Moscow: Labirint.</ref><ref>Peshkov, I. V. (2008). Tsenzura stilya ne rekomenduetsya [Style censorship is not recommended]. In L. S. Vygotskii (Ed.), Psikhologiya iskusstva (pp. 338–340). Moscow: Labirint</ref><ref>Kellogg, D. & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/2011n4a4/2011n4a4.pdf The differences between the Russian and English texts of Tool and Symbol in Child Development. Supplementary and analytic materials] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131220084038/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/2011n4a4/2011n4a4.pdf |date=December 20, 2013 }}. [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php 4(4)] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131220025809/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php |date=December 20, 2013 }}, 98-158</ref><ref>Mecacci, L., & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/2011n4a5/2011n4a5.pdf Editorial Changes in the Three Russian Editions of Vygotsky's Thinking and Speech (1934, 1956, 1982): Towards Authoritative and Ultimate English Translation of the Book] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131220081937/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/2011n4a5/2011n4a5.pdf |date=December 20, 2013 }}. [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php 4(4)] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131220025809/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php |date=December 20, 2013 }}, 159-187</ref> Another series of publications reveals that another well-known Vygotsky's text that is often presented as the foundational work was back-translated into Russian from an English translation of a lost original and passed for the original Vygotsky's writing. This episode was referred to as "benign forgery".<ref>[[Elkhonon Goldberg|Goldberg E.]] [https://books.google.com/books?id=ldWTnKHfJjkC&pg=PA99&lpg=PA99&dq=%22benign+forgery The wisdom paradox: How your mind can grow stronger as your brain grows older]. New York: Gotham, 2005, p. 99</ref><ref>Rieber, R., & Robinson, D. (2004). [https://books.google.com/books?id=uGp6U1WqFi4C&pg=PR16&lpg=PR16&dq=%22The+Russian+original+had+apparently+been+lost%22 Preface. In R. W. Rieber & D. K. Robinson (Eds.), The essential Vygotsky] (pp. xiii-xvii). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.</ref><ref>Goldberg, Е. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.3.1.pdf "Thank you for sharing this fascinating material - very interesting"] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130921141433/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.3.1.pdf |date=September 21, 2013 }} // [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1)] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130204083839/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php |date=February 4, 2013 }}, 118-120</ref><ref>Cole, M. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.4.1.pdf Comments on prior Comments] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130921092917/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.4.1.pdf |date=September 21, 2013 }} // [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1)] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130204083839/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php |date=February 4, 2013 }}, 124-127</ref><ref>van der Veer, R. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.5.1.pdf Rukopisi ne goryat or do they?] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130922010047/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/2012n1a3/2012n1a3.5.1.pdf |date=September 22, 2013 }} // [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php 5 (1), 133-138] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130204083839/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/1/index.php |date=February 4, 2013 }}</ref>

==''Complete Works of L. S. Vygotsky''==
Scholars associated with the [[Historical revisionism|revisionist]] movement in Vygotsky Studies propose returning to Vygotsky's original uncensored works, critically revising the available discourse, and republishing them in both Russian and translation with a rigorous scholarly commentary.<ref name="html"/><ref>[http://www.ichs.udk.-berlin.de Veer, R., van der (2010). Vygotsky in English: What still needs to be done. Website for International Cultural Historical Studies] {{dead link|date=March 2018 |bot=InternetArchiveBot |fix-attempted=yes }})</ref> Therefore, an essential part of this revisionist strand is the ongoing work on "''PsyAnima'' Complete Vygotsky" project that for the first time ever exposes full collections of Vygotsky's texts, uncensored and cleared from numerous mistakes, omissions, insertions, and blatant distortions and falsifications of the author's text made in Soviet editions and uncritically transferred in virtually all foreign translated editions of Vygotsky's works. This project is carried out by an international team of volunteers—researchers, archival workers, and library staff—from Belarus, Brazil, Canada, Israel, Italy, the Netherlands, Russia, and Switzerland, who joined their efforts and put together a collection of L. S. Vygotsky's texts. This publication work is supported by a stream of critical scholarly studies and publications on textology, history, theory and methodology of Vygotskian research that cumulatively contributes to the first ever edition of ''The Complete Works of L.S. Vygotsky''.<ref>Yasnitsky, A. (2012). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/3/2012n3a6/2012n3a6.2.pdf The Complete Works of L.S. Vygotsky: ''PsyAnima Complete Vygotsky'' project] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130319102838/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2012/3/2012n3a6/2012n3a6.2.pdf |date=March 19, 2013 }}. PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 5(3), 144-148</ref> Currently, this collection of Vygotsky's research is available and still in print in a series consisting of six total volumes of his work with added commentary/foreword.

== Vygotsky's scientific bibliography ==
* Van der Veer & Yasnitsky (2016). [https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=iOyPCgAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PP243#v=onepage&q&f=false Vygotsky's published works: a(n almost) definitive bibliography]. In: Yasnitsky, A. & van der Veer, R. (Eds.) (2016). ''[https://www.academia.edu/27328322/Yasnitsky_A._and_Van_der_Veer_R._Eds._2016_._Revisionist_revolution_in_Vygotsky_studies._London_Routledge Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170506123158/http://www.academia.edu/27328322/Yasnitsky_A._and_Van_der_Veer_R._Eds._2016_._Revisionist_revolution_in_Vygotsky_studies._London_Routledge |date=May 6, 2017 }}'' (pp.&nbsp;243–260). London and New York: Routledge


==Works==
==Works==


*Consciousness as a problem in the Psychology of Behavior, essay, 1925
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1925/consciousness.htm Consciousness as a problem in the Psychology of Behavior], 1925
*''Educational Psychology'', 1926
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1926/educational-psychology/index.htm Educational Psychology], 1926
*Historical meaning of the crisis in Psychology, 1927
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/crisis/index.htm Historical meaning of the crisis in Psychology], unfinished and aborted in 1927
*The Problem of the Cultural Development of the Child, essay 1929
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1929/cultural_development.htm The Problem of the Cultural Development of the Child], 1929
*The Fundamental Problems of Defectology, article 1929
*The Fundamental Problems of Defectology, 1929
*The Socialist alteration of Man, 1930
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1930/socialism.htm The Socialist alteration of Man], 1930
*''Ape, Primitive Man, and Child: Essays in the History of Behaviour'', [[Alexander Luria|A. R. Luria]] and L. S. Vygotsky., 1930
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1930/man/index.htm Ape, Primitive Man, and Child: Essays in the History of Behaviour], [[Alexander Luria|A. R. Luria]] and L. S. Vygotsky, 1930
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1934/tool-symbol.htm Tool and symbol in child development], {{circa}}1930
*''Paedology of the Adolescent'', 1931
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1931/adolescent/index.htm Paedology of the Adolescent], 1929-1931
*Play and its role in the Mental development of the Child, essay 1933
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1933/play.htm Play and its role in the Mental development of the Child], oral presentation 1933
*''Thinking and Speech'', 1934
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/words/index.htm Thinking and Speech], 1934
*''Tool and symbol in child development'', 1934
* The Psychology of Art, 1971 (English translation by MIT Press)
*''Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes'', 1978
* Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes, 1978 (Harvard University Press)
*''Thought and Language'', 1986
*''The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky'', 1987 [http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/collected-works.htm overview]
*[https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/collected-works.htm The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky], 1987


==See also==
==See also==
{{cols}}
* [[Cognitivism (learning theory)]]
* [[Cognitivism (learning theory)]]
* [[Cultural-Historical Activity Theory (CHAT)]]
* [[Cultural-Historical Activity Theory (CHAT)]]
* [[Nicola Cuomo]]
* [[Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (LCHC)]]
* [[Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (LCHC)]]
* [[Leading Activity]]
* [[Leading Activity]]
Line 157: Line 170:
* [[Social constructivism]]
* [[Social constructivism]]
* [[Vygotsky Circle]]
* [[Vygotsky Circle]]
{{colend}}


==References==
==References==
{{reflist|3}}
{{reflist|colwidth=30em}}


==Further reading==
==Further reading==
'''Primary'''
*Yasnitsky, A. & van der Veer, R. (Eds.) (2015). [http://www.routledge.com/books/details/9781138887305/ Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies]. Routledge, ISBN 978-1-13-888730-5
*Yasnitsky, A., van der Veer, R., & Ferrari, M. (Eds.) (2014). [http://www.cambridge.org/ca/academic/subjects/psychology/developmental-psychology/cambridge-handbook-cultural-historical-psychology The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
*Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). [https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/handle/1887/10170 Understanding Vygotsky. A quest for synthesis]. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
*Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.) (1994). [https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/handle/1887/11992 The Vygotsky Reader]. Oxford: Blackwell.
*{{cite book| last=Van der Veer| first=Rene| year=2007| title=Lev Vygotsky: Continuum Library of Educational Thought| publisher=Continuum| isbn=0-8264-8409-3}}
*Yasnitsky, A. (2010). [http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2010-15567-001 "Archival revolution" in Vygotskian studies? Uncovering Vygotsky's archives] [http://individual.utoronto.ca/yasnitsky/texts/Yasnitsky%20(2010).%20Arch_Rev.pdf]. Journal of Russian & East European Psychology, Vol 48(1), Jan-Feb 2010, 3-13. {{doi|10.2753/RPO1061-0405480100}}
*Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.scribd.com/doc/79482780/Yasnitsky-2011-Lev-Vygotsky-Philologist-and-Defectologist-Sociointellectual-Biography Lev Vygotsky: Philologist and Defectologist, A Socio-intellectual Biography]. In Pickren, W., Dewsbury, D., & Wertheimer, M. (Eds.). Portraits of Pioneers in Developmental Psychology, vol. VII.
*van der Veer, R. & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.springerlink.com/content/278j5025767m2263/ Vygotsky in English: What Still Needs to Be Done]. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science [http://www.springerlink.com/content/278j5025767m2263/fulltext.html html], [http://www.springerlink.com/content/278j5025767m2263/fulltext.pdf pdf]
*Yasnitsky, A. (2012). [http://individual.utoronto.ca/yasnitsky/texts/Yasnitsky%20(2012).%20Revisionist_revolution.pdf Revisionist Revolution in Vygotskian Science: Toward Cultural-Historical Gestalt Psychology]. Guest Editor’s Introduction. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 50(4), 3-15. DOI: 10.2753/RPO1061-0405500400

'''Secondary'''
*Kozulin, A. (1990). Vygotsky's Psychology: A Biography of Ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
*Vygodskaya, G. L., & Lifanova, T. M. (1996/1999). Lev Semenovich Vygotsky, Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, Part 1, 37 (2), 3-90; Part 2, 37 (3), 3-90; Part 3, 37 (4), 3-93, Part 4, 37 (5), 3-99.
*Veresov, N. N. (1999). Undiscovered Vygotsky: Etudes on the pre-history of cultural-historical psychology. New York: Peter Lang.
*Daniels, H., Wertsch, J. & Cole, M. (Eds.) (2007). [http://www.amazon.com/dp/0521537878 The Cambridge Companion to Vygotsky].
*Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.springerlink.com/content/b34101p383588v95/ Vygotsky Circle as a Personal Network of Scholars: Restoring Connections Between People and Ideas] ([http://www.scribd.com/doc/71468002/Yasnitsky-2011-Vygotsky-Circle ''idem'']). Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, {{doi|10.1007/s12124-011-9168-5}} [http://individual.utoronto.ca/yasnitsky/texts/Yasnitsky%20(2011).%20Vygotsky%20Circle.pdf ''pdf'']
* Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/2011n4a1/2011n4a1.1.pdf The Vygotsky That We (Do Not) Know: Vygotsky’s Main Works and the Chronology of their Composition]. [[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]], [http://www.psyanima.ru/journal/2011/4/index.php 4(4)]

==External links==
{{Library resources box|by=yes|onlinebooks=no|about=yes|wikititle=Lev Vygotsky}}
{{Library resources box|by=yes|onlinebooks=no|about=yes|wikititle=Lev Vygotsky}}


=== Primary ===
*{{Wikiquote-inline}}
* Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.) (1994). [https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/handle/1887/11992 The Vygotsky Reader]. Oxford: Blackwell.
*[http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/ Lev Vygotsky archive, marxists.org]: all major works
* Van der Veer, R., & [[Jaan Valsiner|Valsiner, J.]] (1991). [https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/handle/1887/10170 Understanding Vygotsky. A quest for synthesis]. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
*[http://webpages.charter.net/schmolze1/vygotsky/ The Vygotsky Project] Summaries of, and links to, Vygotsky articles.
*[http://vygotsky.afraid.org/ The Mozart of Psychology] Vygotsky article with extensive references.
*[http://www.vygotsky-robbins.com Dorothy "Dot" Robbins] Vygotsky memorial site with many papers and resources.
*[http://lchc.ucsd.edu/MCA/Paper/index.html XMCA Research Paper Archive] Various articles on Vygotskian psychology
*Garai, L. Another crisis in the psychology: [http://www.staff.u-szeged.hu/~garai/Vygotskyboom.htm A possible motive for the Vygotsky-boom]
*Garai, L. [http://www.staff.u-szeged.hu/~garai/Vymplic.htm Vygotskian implications: On the meaning and its brain]
*[http://ahp.yorku.ca/?p=387 Annotated bibliography of scholarly histories on Vygotsky], Advances in the History of Psychology, [[York University]]
* L. Miffre, To form with Vygotstki. Psychology of the activity of teacher in situation, Presses Universitaires de Bordeaux, May 2013, [http://pub.u-bordeaux3.fr/index.php/nouveautes/se-former-avec-vygotski-psychologie-de-l-activite-du-maitre-en-situation.html]
* [http://www.toolsofthemind.org/philosophy/vygotskian-approach/ Tools of the Mind]


=== Secondary ===
{{Philosophy of language}}
* Kozulin, A. (1990). Vygotsky's Psychology: A Biography of Ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
* Vygodskaya, G. L., & Lifanova, T. M. (1996/1999). Lev Semenovich Vygotsky, Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, Part 1, 37 (2), 3-90; Part 2, 37 (3), 3-90; Part 3, 37 (4), 3-93, Part 4, 37 (5), 3-99.
* {{cite book| last=Van der Veer| first=Rene| year=2007| title=Lev Vygotsky: Continuum Library of Educational Thought| publisher=Continuum| isbn=978-0-8264-8409-3}}
* Daniels, H., Wertsch, J. & Cole, M. (Eds.) (2007). [https://www.amazon.com/dp/0521537878 The Cambridge Companion to Vygotsky].
* Dafermos, M. (2018). Cultural-Historical Theory A Dialectical Perspective to Vygotsky.Singapore: Springer.
* Guilherme, Alexandre and Morgan, W. John, (2018), 'Lev S. Vygotsky (1896-1934)-dialogue as meditation and inner speech', Chapter 3 in ''Philosophy, Dialogue, and Education: Nine modern European philosophers'', Routledge, London and New York, pp.&nbsp;39–54, {{ISBN|978-1-138-83149-0}}.
* {{cite journal | last1 = Yasnitsky | first1 = A | year = 2011 | title = Vygotsky Circle as a Personal Network of Scholars: Restoring Connections Between People and Ideas | journal = Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science | volume = 45| issue = 4| pages = 422–457| doi = 10.1007/s12124-011-9168-5 | pmid = 21667127 | s2cid = 207392569 }}
* {{cite journal | last1 = van der Veer | first1 = R. | last2 = Yasnitsky | first2 = A. | year = 2011 | title = Vygotsky in English: What Still Needs to Be Done | journal = Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science | volume = 45| issue = 4| pages = 475–493 | doi = 10.1007/s12124-011-9172-9 | pmid = 21626141 | pmc = 3181411 }}
* Yasnitsky, A. (2011). [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/2011n4a1/2011n4a1.1.pdf The Vygotsky That We (Do Not) Know: Vygotsky's Main Works and the Chronology of their Composition]. ''[[PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal]]'', [http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php 4(4)] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131220025809/http://www.psyanima.su/journal/2011/4/index.php |date=December 20, 2013 }}


==External links==
* {{Commons category-inline}}
* {{Wikiquote-inline}}
* [http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/ Lev Vygotsky archive, marxists.org]: all major works
* Garai, L. Another crisis in the psychology: [http://www.staff.u-szeged.hu/~garai/Vygotskyboom.htm A possible motive for the Vygotsky-boom]
* [http://ahp.yorku.ca/?p=387 Annotated bibliography of scholarly histories on Vygotsky], Advances in the History of Psychology, [[York University]]
{{Human psychological development}}
{{Philosophy of language}}
{{Authority control}}
{{Authority control}}


{{Persondata <!-- Metadata: see [[Wikipedia:Persondata]]. -->
| NAME = Vygotsky, Lev
| ALTERNATIVE NAMES =
| SHORT DESCRIPTION = Soviet psychologist
| DATE OF BIRTH = 1896
| PLACE OF BIRTH = [[Orsha]], [[Russian Empire]]
| DATE OF DEATH = June 11, 1934
| PLACE OF DEATH = [[Moscow]], [[USSR]]
}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Vygotsky, Lev}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Vygotsky, Lev}}
[[Category:1896 births]]
[[Category:1896 births]]
[[Category:1934 deaths]]
[[Category:1934 deaths]]
[[Category:People from Orsha]]
[[Category:People from Orsha]]
[[Category:People from Mogilev Governorate]]
[[Category:People from Orshansky Uyezd]]
[[Category:Belarusian Jews]]
[[Category:Belarusian Jews]]
[[Category:Soviet Jews]]
[[Category:Belarusian scientists]]
[[Category:Belarusian scientists]]
[[Category:Developmental psychologists]]
[[Category:Educational psychologists]]
[[Category:Soviet scientists]]
[[Category:Soviet psychologists]]
[[Category:Systems psychologists]]
[[Category:Cognitive scientists]]
[[Category:Cognitive scientists]]
[[Category:Cognitive psychologists]]
[[Category:Communication theorists]]
[[Category:Communication theorists]]
[[Category:Constructivism (psychological school)]]
[[Category:Developmental psychologists]]
[[Category:Educational psychologists]]
[[Category:Literacy and society theorists]]
[[Category:Literacy and society theorists]]
[[Category:Philosophers of education]]
[[Category:Philosophers of education]]
[[Category:Linguistic turn]]
[[Category:Soviet psychologists]]
[[Category:Soviet scientists]]
[[Category:Jewish Russian scientists]]
[[Category:Spinoza scholars]]
[[Category:Jewish philosophers]]
[[Category:Spinozists]]
[[Category:Systems psychologists]]
[[Category:Moscow State University alumni]]
[[Category:Moscow State University alumni]]
[[Category:Deaths from tuberculosis]]
[[Category:20th-century deaths from tuberculosis]]
[[Category:Moscow State University faculty]]
[[Category:Imperial Moscow University alumni]]
[[Category:20th-century psychologists]]
[[Category:Tuberculosis deaths in the Soviet Union]]
[[Category:Tuberculosis deaths in Russia]]

Revision as of 12:29, 28 November 2022

Lev Vygotsky
Born
Lev Simkhovich Výgodsky

(1896-11-17)November 17, 1896
Orsha, Russian Empire, now in Belarus
DiedJune 11, 1934(1934-06-11) (aged 37)
Moscow, Soviet Union
Alma materImperial Moscow University (1917) (unfinished);
Shaniavskii Moscow City People's University
Known forCultural-historical psychology, zone of proximal development, inner speech
SpouseRoza Noevna Vygodskaia (née Smekhova)
Scientific career
FieldsPsychology
InstitutionsMoscow State University
Thesis The Psychology of Art  (1925)
Notable studentsAlexander Luria

Lev Semyonovich Vygotsky (Russian: Лев Семёнович Выго́тский; Belarusian: Леў Сямёнавіч Выго́цкі; November 17 [O.S. November 5] 1896 – June 11, 1934) was a Soviet psychologist, known for his work on psychological development in children. He published on a diverse range of subjects, and from multiple views as his perspective changed over the years. Among his students was Alexander Luria and Kharkiv school of psychology.

He is known for his concept of the zone of proximal development (ZPD): the distance between what a student (apprentice, new employee, etc.) can do on their own, and what they can accomplish with the support of someone more knowledgeable about the activity. Vygotsky saw the ZPD as a measure of skills that are in the process of maturing, as supplement to measures of development that only look at a learner's independent ability.

Also influential are his works on the relationship between language and thought, the development of language, and a general theory of development through actions and relationships in a socio-cultural environment. This can be found in many of his essays.

Overview of scientific legacy

Despite his claim for a "new psychology" that he foresaw as a "science of the Superman" of the Communist future,[1][2][3][4] Vygotsky's main work was in developmental psychology. In order to fully understand the human mind, he believed one must understand its genesis. Consequently, the majority of his work involved the study of infant and child behavior, as well as the development of language acquisition (such as the importance of pointing and inner speech[5]) and the development of concepts; now often referred to as schemas or schemata.[6][7][8]

Early in the psychological research period of his career (1920s), which focused upon mechanistic and reductionist "instrumental psychology" in many ways inspired by the work of Ivan Pavlov (his theory of "higher nervous activity") and Vladimir Bekhterev (and his "reflexologist" followers), Vygotsky argued that human psychological development could be formed through the use of meaningless (i.e., virtually random) signs that he viewed as the psychological equivalent of instrument use in human labor and industry.[9] It was later during the "holistic" period of his career (the first half of the 1930s) that Vygotsky rejected this earlier reductionist views on signs.

While Vygotsky never met Jean Piaget, he had read a number of his works and agreed on some of his perspectives on learning.[10] At some point (around 1929–30), Vygotsky came to disagree with Piaget's understanding of learning and development, and held a different theoretical position from Piaget on the topic of inner speech; Piaget asserted that egocentric speech in children "dissolved away" as they matured, while Vygotsky maintained that egocentric speech became internalized, what we now call "inner speech".[11] However, in the early 1930s he radically changed his mind on Piaget's theory and openly praised him for his discovery of the social origin of children's speech, reasoning, and moral judgements. Piaget only read Vygotsky's work after his death.[10]

Nearing the end of his life, Vygotsky's later work involved adolescent development.[12] However, his most important and widely known contribution is his theory for the development of "higher psychological functions," which considers human psychological development as emerging through unification of interpersonal connections and actions taken within a given socio-cultural environment (i.e., language, culture, society, and tool-use). Vygotsky eventually came to dialogue with the mainstream Gestalt line of thought and adopted a more holistic approach to understanding development. Under the increasing influence of the holistic thinking of the scholars primarily associated with the German-American Gestalt psychology movement, Vygotsky adopted their views on "psychological systems" and—inspired by Kurt Lewin's "topological (and vector) psychology"—introduced the enigmatic construct of the "zone of proximal development". It was during this period that he identified the play of young children as their "leading activity", which he understood to be the main source of preschoolers' psychological development, and which he viewed as an expression of an inseparable unity of emotional, volitional, and cognitive development. At this time, Vygotsky fully revealed his long-time interest in the philosophy of Spinoza, who would remain one of his favorite thinkers throughout his life. A fervent Spinozist in many respects, Vygotsky was profoundly influenced by Spinoza's thought, largely in response to Spinoza's examinations concerning human emotion.[13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20][21] As his work matured, Spinoza's thought became a more central visitation in Vygotsky's later work, increasingly focused on the issue of human emotion and its role in higher psychological functions and development that he largely omitted in his earlier work and utterly needed for creating a holistic psychological theory.

As early as the mid-1920s, Vygotsky's ideas were introduced in the West, but he remained virtually unknown until the early 1980s when the popularity among educators of the constructivist developmental psychology and educational theory of Jean Piaget (1896-1980) started to decline and, in contrast, Vygotsky's notion of the "zone of proximal development" became a central component of the development of "social constructivist" turn in developmental and, primarily, educational psychology and practice. A Review of General Psychology study, published in 2002, ranked Vygotsky as the 83rd top psychologist of the twentieth century and the third (and the last) Russian on the top-100 list after Ivan Pavlov and Vygotsky's longtime collaborator Alexander Luria.[22]

Biography

Lev Semionovich Vygotsky (Russian: Лев Семёнович Выго́тский, IPA: [vɨˈɡotskʲɪj]; November 17 [O.S. November 5] 1896 – June 11, 1934) was born to the Vygodskii family in the town of Orsha, Belarus (then belonging to the Russian Empire) into a non-religious middle-class family of Russian Jewish extraction.[23] His father Simkha Vygodskii was a banker.

Vygodskii was raised in the city of Gomel, where he was homeschooled until 1911 and then obtained a formal degree with distinction in a private Jewish gymnasium, which allowed him entrance to a university. In 1913 Vygodskii was admitted to the Moscow University by mere ballot through a "Jewish Lottery": at the time a three percent Jewish student quota was administered for entry in Moscow and Saint Petersburg universities. He had interest in humanities and social sciences, but at the insistence of his parents he applied to the medical school in Moscow University. During the first semester of study he transferred to the law school. There he studied law and, in parallel, he attended lectures at Shaniavskii University.

Vygodskii's early interests were in the arts and, primarily, in the topics of the history of the Jewish people, the tradition, culture and Jewish identity. In contrast, during this period he was highly critical of the ideas of both socialism and Zionism, and proposed the solution of the "Jewish question" by return to the traditional Jewish Orthodoxy. His own academics, however, included a wide field of studies including linguistics, psychology, and philosophy.

Interrupted by the October Bolshevik uprising in 1917, Vygotsky never completed his formal studies at the Imperial Moscow University, and thus, he never obtained a degree. Following these events, he left Moscow and eventually returned to Gomel. There is virtually no information about his life when Gomel was under German occupation (administratively belonging to the Ukrainian State at the time) during World War I, until the Bolsheviks captured the city in 1919. After that, he was an active participant of major social transformation under the Bolshevik (Communist) rule and a fairly prominent representative of the Bolshevik government in Gomel from 1919 to 1923. By the early 1920s, as reflected in his journalistic publications of the time, he informally changed his Jewish-sounding birth name, 'Lev Símkhovich Výgodskii' (Russian: Лев Си́мхович Вы́годский), with the surname becoming Vygótskii and the patronymic 'Símkhovich' becoming the Slavic 'Semiónovich'. It was under this pen-name that the fame subsequently came to him. His daughters (subsequently born in 1925 and 1930) and other relatives, though, preserved their original family name 'Vygodskii'. The traditional English spelling of his last name nowadays is 'Vygotsky'.[24]

In January 1924, Vygotsky took part in the Second All-Russian Psychoneurological Congress in Petrograd (soon thereafter renamed Leningrad). After the Congress, Vygotsky met with Alexander Luria and with his help received an invitation to become a research fellow at the Psychological Institute in Moscow which was under the direction of Konstantin Kornilov. Vygotsky moved to Moscow with his new wife, Roza Smekhova. He began his career at the Psychological Institute as a "staff scientist, second class".[25] He also became a secondary teacher, covering a period marked by his interest in the processes of learning and the role of language in learning.[26]

By the end of 1925, Vygotsky completed his dissertation titled "The Psychology of Art", that was not published until the 1960s, and a book titled "Pedagogical Psychology", that apparently was created on the basis of lecture notes that he prepared in Gomel while he was a psychology instructor at local educational establishments. In the summer of 1925 he made his first and only trip abroad to a London congress on the education of the deaf.[27] Upon return to the Soviet Union, he was hospitalized due to tuberculosis and, having miraculously survived, would remain an invalid and out of work until the end of 1926.[28][29] His dissertation was accepted as the prerequisite of a scholarly degree, which was awarded to Vygotsky in autumn 1925 in absentia.

After his release from the hospital, Vygotsky did theoretical and methodological work on the crisis in psychology, but never finished the draft of the manuscript and interrupted his work on it around mid-1927. The manuscript was published later with notable editorial interventions and distortions in 1982 and was presented by the editors as one of the most important of Vygotsky's works.[30][31][32][33][34] In this early manuscript, Vygotsky argued for the formation of a general psychology that could unite the naturalist objectivist strands of psychological science with the more philosophical approaches of Marxist orientation. However, he also harshly criticized those of his colleagues who attempted to build a "Marxist Psychology" as an alternative to the naturalist and philosophical schools. He argued that if one wanted to build a truly Marxist Psychology, there were no shortcuts to be found by merely looking for applicable quotes in the writings of Marx. Rather one should look for a methodology that was in accordance with the Marxian spirit.[35]

From 1926 to 1930, Vygotsky worked on a research program investigating the development of higher cognitive functions of logical memory, selective attention, decision making, and language comprehension, from early forms of primal psychological functions. During this period he gathered a group of collaborators including Alexander Luria, Boris Varshava, Alexei Leontiev, Leonid Zankov, and several others. Vygotsky guided his students in researching this phenomenon from three different perspectives:

  • The instrumental approach, which aimed to understand the ways humans use objects as mediation aids in memory and reasoning.
  • A developmental approach, focused on how children acquire higher cognitive functions during development
  • A culture-historical approach, studying how social and cultural patterns of interaction shape forms of mediation and developmental trajectories [35]

In the early 1930s, Vygotsky experienced deep crises, both personal and theoretical, and after a period of massive self-criticism, he made an attempt at a radical revision of his theory. The work of the representatives of the Gestalt psychology and other holistic scholars was instrumental in this theoretical shift. In 1932–1934, Vygotsky aimed to establish a psychological theory of consciousness, but because of his death, this theory remained only unconfirmed and unfinished.

Life and scientific legacy

Vygotsky was a pioneering psychologist and his major works span six separate volumes, written over roughly ten years, from Psychology of Art (1925) to Thought and Language [or Thinking and Speech] (1934). Vygotsky's interests in the fields of developmental psychology, child development, and education were extremely diverse. His philosophical framework includes interpretations of the cognitive role of mediation tools, as well as the re-interpretation of well-known concepts in psychology such as internalization of knowledge. Vygotsky introduced the notion of zone of proximal development, a metaphor capable of describing the potential of human cognitive development. His work covered topics such as the origin and the psychology of art, development of higher mental functions, philosophy of science and the methodology of psychological research, the relation between learning and human development, concept formation, interrelation between language and thought development, play as a psychological phenomenon, learning disabilities, and abnormal human development (aka defectology). His scientific thinking underwent several major transformations throughout his career, but generally Vygotsky's legacy may be divided into two fairly distinct periods,[citation needed] and the transitional phase between the two during which Vygotsky experienced the crisis in his theory and personal life. These are the mechanistic "instrumental" period of the 1920s, integrative "holistic" period of the 1930s, and the transitional years of, roughly, 1929–1931. Each of these periods is characterized by its distinct themes and theoretical innovations.

"Instrumental" period (1920s)

Cultural mediation and internalization

Vygotsky studied child development and the significant roles of cultural mediation and interpersonal communication. He observed how higher mental functions developed through these interactions, and also represented the shared knowledge of a culture. This process is known as internalization. Internalization may be understood in one respect as "knowing how". For example, the practices of riding a bicycle or pouring a cup of milk, initially, are outside and beyond the child. The mastery of the skills needed for performing these practices occurs through the activity of the child within society. A further aspect of internalization is appropriation, in which children take tools and adapt them to personal use, perhaps using them in unique ways. Internalizing the use of a pencil allows the child to use it very much for personal ends rather than drawing exactly what others in society have drawn previously.

The period of crisis, criticism, and self-criticism (1929–1932)

In the 1930s, Vygotsky was engaged in massive reconstruction of the theory of his "instrumental" period of the 1920s. Around 1929–1930, he realized numerous deficiencies and imperfections of the earlier work of the Vygotsky Circle and criticized it on a number of occasions: in 1929,[36] 1930,[37] in 1931,[38] and in 1932.[39] Specifically, Vygotsky criticized his earlier idea of radical separation between the "lower" and "higher" psychological functions and, around 1932, appears to abandon it.[40]

Vygotsky's self-criticism was complemented by external criticism for a number of issues, including the separation between the "higher" and the "lower" psychological functions, impracticality and inapplicability of his theory in social practices (such as industry or education) during the time of rapid social change, and vulgar Marxist interpretation of human psychological processes. Critics also pointed to his overemphasis on the role of language and, on the other hand, the ignorance of the emotional factors in human development. Major figures in Soviet psychology such as Sergei Rubinstein criticized Vygotsky's notion of mediation and its development in the works of students. Following criticism and in response to a generous offer from the highest officials in Soviet Ukraine, a major group of Vygotsky's associates, the members of the Vygotsky Circle, including Luria, Mark Lebedinsky, and Leontiev, moved from Moscow to Ukraine to establish the Kharkov school of psychology. In the second half of the 1930s, Vygotsky was criticized again for his involvement in the cross-disciplinary study of the child known as paedology and uncritical borrowings from contemporary "bourgeois" science. Considerable critique came from alleged followers of Vygotsky, such as Leontiev and members of his research group in Kharkov. Much of this early criticism was later discarded by these Vygotskian scholars as well.

"Holistic" period (1932–1934)

There occurred a period of major revision of Vygotsky's theory, a transition from a mechanist orientation of his 1920s to an integrative holistic science of the 1930s. During this period, Vygotsky was under particularly strong influence of holistic theories of German-American group of proponents of Gestalt psychology, most notably, the peripheral participants of the Gestalt movement Kurt Goldstein and Kurt Lewin. However, Vygotsky's work of this period remained largely fragmentary and unfinished and, therefore, unpublished.

Zone of Proximal Development

"Zone of Proximal Development" (ZPD) is a term Vygotsky used to characterize an individual's mental development. He originally defined the ZPD as “the distance between the actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers.” He used the example of two children in school who originally could solve problems at an eight-year-old developmental level (that is, typical for children who were age 8). After each child received assistance from an adult, one was able to perform at a nine-year-old level and one was able to perform at a twelve-year-old level. He said "This difference between twelve and eight, or between nine and eight, is what we call the zone of proximal development." He further said that the ZPD “defines those functions that have not yet matured but are in the process of maturation, functions that will mature tomorrow but are currently in an embryonic state.” The zone is bracketed by the learner's current ability and the ability they can achieve with the aid of an instructor of some capacity.[41]

Vygotsky viewed the ZPD as a better way to explain the relation between children's learning and cognitive development. Prior to the ZPD, the relation between learning and development could be boiled down to the following three major positions: 1) Development always precedes learning (e.g., constructivism): children first need to meet a particular maturation level before learning can occur; 2) Learning and development cannot be separated, but instead occur simultaneously (e.g., behaviorism): essentially, learning is development; and 3) learning and development are separate, but interactive processes (e.g., gestaltism): one process always prepares the other process, and vice versa. Vygotsky rejected these three major theories because he believed that learning should always precede development in the ZPD. According to Vygotsky, through the assistance of a more knowledgeable other, a child is able to learn skills or aspects of a skill that go beyond the child's actual developmental or maturational level. The lower limit of ZPD is the level of skill reached by the child working independently (also referred to as the child's developmental level). The upper limit is the level of potential skill that the child is able to reach with the assistance of a more capable instructor. In this sense, the ZPD provides a prospective view of cognitive development, as opposed to a retrospective view that characterizes development in terms of a child's independent capabilities. The advancement through and attainment of the upper limit of the ZPD is limited by the instructional and scaffolding-related capabilities of the more knowledgeable other (MKO). The MKO is typically assumed to be an older, more experienced teacher or parent, but often can be a learner's peer or someone their junior. The MKO need not even be a person, it can be a machine or book, or other source of visual and/or audio input.[42]

Thinking and speech

Perhaps Vygotsky's most important contribution concerns the inter-relationship of language development and thought. This problem was explored in Vygotsky's book, Thinking and speech, entitled in Russian, Myshlenie i rech (Мышление и речь), that was published in 1934. In fact, this book was a mere collection of essays and scholarly papers that Vygotsky wrote during different periods of his thought development and included writings of his "instrumental" and "holistic" periods. Vygotsky never saw the book published: it was published posthumously, edited by his closest associates (Kolbanovskii, Zankov, and Shif) not sooner than December 1934, i.e., half a year after his death. The first English translation was published in 1962 (with several later revised editions) heavily abbreviated and under an alternative and incorrect translation of the title Thought and Language for the Russian title Mysl' i iazyk. The book establishes the explicit and profound connection between speech (both silent inner speech and oral language), and the development of mental concepts and cognitive awareness. Vygotsky described inner speech as being qualitatively different from verbal external speech. Although Vygotsky believed inner speech developed from external speech via a gradual process of "internalization" (i.e., transition from the external to the internal), with younger children only really able to "think out loud", he claimed that in its mature form, inner speech would not resemble spoken language as we know it (in particular, being greatly compressed). Hence, thought itself developing socially.

Death (1934) and posthumous fame

Vygotsky died of a relapse of tuberculosis on June 11, 1934, at the age of 37, in Moscow in the Soviet Union. One of Vygotsky's last private notebook entries gives a proverbial, yet very pessimistic self-assessment of his contribution to psychological theory:

This is the final thing I have done in psychology – and I will like Moses die at the summit, having glimpsed the promised land but without setting foot on it. Farewell, dear creations. The rest is silence.[9]

Immediately after his death, Vygotsky was proclaimed one of the leading psychologists in the Soviet Union, although his stellar reputation was somewhat undermined by the decree of the Central Committee of the Communist Party of 1936 that denounced the mass movement, discipline, and related social practice of the so-called pedology. Yet, even despite some criticisms and censorship of his works — most notably, in the post-Stalin era by his self-proclaimed best students and followers — Vygotsky always remained among the most quoted scholars in the field and has become a cult figure for a number of contemporary intellectuals and practitioners in Russia and the international psychological and educational community alike.[43][44]

Influence worldwide

Eastern Europe

In the Soviet Union, the work of the group of Vygotsky's students known as the Vygotsky Circle was responsible for Vygotsky's scientific legacy.[45] The members of the group subsequently laid a foundation for Vygotskian psychology's systematic development in such diverse fields as the psychology of memory (P. Zinchenko), perception, sensation, and movement (Zaporozhets, Asnin, A. N. Leont'ev), personality (Lidiya Bozhovich, Asnin, A. N. Leont'ev), will and volition (Zaporozhets, A. N. Leont'ev, P. Zinchenko, L. Bozhovich, Asnin), psychology of play (G. D. Lukov, Daniil El'konin) and psychology of learning (P. Zinchenko, L. Bozhovich, D. El'konin), as well as the theory of step-by-step formation of mental actions (Pyotr Gal'perin), general psychological activity theory (A. N. Leont'ev) and psychology of action (Zaporozhets).[45] Andrey Puzyrey elaborated the ideas of Vygotsky in respect of psychotherapy and even in the broader context of deliberate psychological intervention (psychotechnique), in general.[46] Laszlo Garai[47] founded a Vygotskian research group.

North America

In North America, Vygotsky's work was known from the end of the 1920s through a series of publications in English, but it did not have a major effect on research in general; in fact many scholars have stressed the lack of application to contemporary psychological research.[48] In 1962 a translation of his posthumous 1934 book, Thinking and Speech, published with the title,Thought and Language, did not seem to change the situation considerably.[citation needed] It was only after an eclectic compilation of partly rephrased and partly translated works of Vygotsky and his collaborators, published in 1978 under Vygotsky's name as Mind in Society, that the Vygotsky boom started in the West: originally, in North America, and later, following the North American example, spread to other regions of the world. A lot of Vigotsky's principles are taught education in today's society.[49]This version of Vygotskian science is typically associated with the names of its chief proponents Michael Cole, James Wertsch, their associates and followers, and is relatively well known under the names of "cultural-historical activity theory" (aka CHAT) or "activity theory".[50][51][52] Scaffolding, a concept introduced by Wood, Bruner, and Ross in 1976, is somewhat related to the idea of ZPD, although Vygotsky never used the term.[53]

Criticisms of North American "Vygotskian" and original Vygotsky's legacy

A critique of the North American interpretation of Vygotsky's ideas and, somewhat later, its global spread and dissemination appeared in the 1980s.[54] The early 1980s criticism of Russian and Western "Vygotskian" scholars [55] continued throughout the 1990s. Van der Veer & Valsiner (1991) called these strands of Vygotskian thought "neo-Vygotskian fashions in contemporary psychology"[56] and Cazden (1996) called them "selective traditions" in Vygotskian scholarship.[57] Palincsar (1998) said that the zone of proximal development was "one of the most used and least understood constructs to appear in contemporary educational literature",[58] and Fischer & Mercer (1992) said that the construct was "used as little more than a fashionable alternative to Piagetian terminology or the concept of IQ for describing individual differences in attainment or potential".[59]

Van der Veer and Valsiner also suggest clearly distinguishing between Vygotsky's original notion of "zona blizhaishego razvitiia" (ZBR) and what they think of as its "superficial interpretations" collectively known as "zone of proximal development" (ZPD).[60][61] Valsiner and Van der Veer also identify certain statements in Vygotskian literature as mere "declarations of faith", i.e. hollow statements often repeated.[62] Whereas Gillen (2000) talks of different "versions of Vygotsky"[63] and Van der Veer (2008) speaks of "multiple readings of Vygotsky",[64] Gradler (2007) simply speaks of "concepts and inferences curiously attributed to Lev Vygotsky".[65] Toomela (2000) calls the strand known as "activity theory" a "dead end” for cultural-historical psychology [51] and, moreover, for methodological thinking in cultural psychology.[52]

Gredler & Schields (2004) question "if anyone actually reads Vygotsky's words"[66] and Gredler (2012) asks whether it is "too late to understand Vygotsky for the classroom",[67] while Rowlands (2000) suggests "turning Vygotsky on his head".[68] According to Miller (2011), inconsistencies, contradictions, and at times fundamental flaws in "Vygotskian" literature were revealed in critical publications on this subject and are typically associated with - but certainly not limited to - the North American legacy of Michael Cole and James Wertsch and their associates.[69] Smagorinsky (2011) speaks of "challenges of claiming a Vygotskian perspective".[70]

Lambert (2000) claims that Vygotsky's writings on play were too brief to be called a theory and furthermore were hardly relevant anymore.[71] According to Zhang (2013), Vygotsky's theories are fundamentally flawed from a contemporary linguistic standpoint[72][73] and Newman (2018) claims that "the support claimed from Vygotsky in accounts of second language acquisition is misplaced, first because of those difficulties and, second, because many who claim support from Vygotsky, do not need or even use his theory but instead focus their attention on his empirical observations and assume incorrectly that if their own empirical observations match Vygotsky's, then Vygotsky's theory can be accepted".[74]

Revisionist movement in Vygotsky Studies

During the early twenty-first century, several scholarly reevaluations of the popular version (sometimes disparagingly termed "Vygotsky cult", "the cult of Vygotsky", or even "the cult of personality around Vygotsky") of Vygotsky's legacy have been undertaken and are referred to as the "revisionist revolution in Vygotsky Studies".[9][75][76] Vygotsky studies conducted within the framework of the "revisionist turn" during the 2010s revealed systematic and massive falsifications and distortions of Vygotsky's legacy., but also demonstrated a rapid and dramatic decrease of this author's popularity in international scholarship that began in 2017.[citation needed] The reasons of this crisis are not entirely clear yet and are being discussed in scholarly circles.[1][77]

The revisionist movement in Vygotsky Studies was termed a "revisionist revolution"[9] to describe a relatively recent trend that emerged in the 1990s. This trend is typically associated with growing dissatisfaction with the quality and scholarly integrity of available texts of Vygotsky and members of Vygotsky Circle, including their English translations made from largely mistaken, distorted, and even in a few instances falsified Soviet editions,[78][79] which raises serious concerns about the reliability of Vygotsky's texts available in English.[80] However, unlike critical literature that discusses Western interpretations of Vygotsky's legacy, the target of criticism and the primary object of research in the studies of the revisionist strand are Vygotsky's texts proper: the manuscripts, original lifetime publications, and Vygotsky's posthumous Soviet editions that most often were subsequently uncritically translated into other languages. The revisionist strand is solidly grounded in a series of studies in Vygotsky's archives that uncovered previously unknown and unpublished Vygotsky materials.[30][81][82][83][84][85][86][87][88]

Thus, some studies of the revisionist strand show that certain phrases, terms, and expressions typically associated with Vygotskian legacy as its core notions and concepts—such as "cultural-historical psychology",[89][90][91][92] "cultural-historical theory", "cultural-historical school", "higher psychical/mental functions", "internalization", "zone of proximal development", etc. in fact, either occupy not more than just a few dozen pages within the six-volume collection of Vygotsky's works,[93][94] or never even occur in Vygotsky's own writings.[95] Another series of studies revealed the questionable quality of Vygotsky's published texts that, in fact, were never finished and intended for publication by their author,[31][32][96] but were nevertheless posthumously published without giving proper editorial acknowledgement of their unfinished, transitory nature,[33][97] and with numerous editorial interventions and distortions of Vygotsky's text.[98][99][100][101][102][103][104] Another series of publications reveals that another well-known Vygotsky's text that is often presented as the foundational work was back-translated into Russian from an English translation of a lost original and passed for the original Vygotsky's writing. This episode was referred to as "benign forgery".[105][106][107][108][109]

Complete Works of L. S. Vygotsky

Scholars associated with the revisionist movement in Vygotsky Studies propose returning to Vygotsky's original uncensored works, critically revising the available discourse, and republishing them in both Russian and translation with a rigorous scholarly commentary.[80][110] Therefore, an essential part of this revisionist strand is the ongoing work on "PsyAnima Complete Vygotsky" project that for the first time ever exposes full collections of Vygotsky's texts, uncensored and cleared from numerous mistakes, omissions, insertions, and blatant distortions and falsifications of the author's text made in Soviet editions and uncritically transferred in virtually all foreign translated editions of Vygotsky's works. This project is carried out by an international team of volunteers—researchers, archival workers, and library staff—from Belarus, Brazil, Canada, Israel, Italy, the Netherlands, Russia, and Switzerland, who joined their efforts and put together a collection of L. S. Vygotsky's texts. This publication work is supported by a stream of critical scholarly studies and publications on textology, history, theory and methodology of Vygotskian research that cumulatively contributes to the first ever edition of The Complete Works of L.S. Vygotsky.[111] Currently, this collection of Vygotsky's research is available and still in print in a series consisting of six total volumes of his work with added commentary/foreword.

Vygotsky's scientific bibliography

Works

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Yasnitsky, A. (2018). Vygotsky's science of Superman: from Utopia to concrete psychology. In Yasnitsky, A. (Ed.). (2018). Questioning Vygotsky's Legacy: Scientific Psychology or Heroic Cult. London & New York: Routledge.
  2. ^ Dr. Clay Spinuzzi blog, book review: New Myth, New World: From Nietzsche to Stalinism
  3. ^ Spinuzzi, C. (2018). From superhumans to supermediators: Locating the extraordinary in CHAT. In A. Yasnitsky (Ed.), Questioning Vygotsky's legacy: Scientific psychology or heroic cult (pp. 137–166). New York, NY: Routledge.
  4. ^ Zavershneva, E. (2014). The problem of consciousness in Vygotsky's cultural-historical psychology. In A. Yasnitsky, R. Van der Veer & M. Ferrari (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology, 63-97. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  5. ^ Alderson-Day, Ben; Fernyhough, Charles (2015). "Inner Speech: Development, Cognitive Functions, Phenomenology, and Neurobiology". Psychological Bulletin. 141 (5): 931–965. doi:10.1037/bul0000021. PMC 4538954. PMID 26011789.
  6. ^ Vygotsky, L.S. Thought and Language (1932). Chapter 6: The Development of Scientific Concepts in Childhood. marxists.org
  7. ^ Oxford Reference: Vygotsky blocks
  8. ^ Paula Towsey on the Blocks Experiment (2008) Vimeo.com
  9. ^ a b c d Yasnitsky, A. & van der Veer, R. (Eds.) (2015). Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies. London and New York: Routledge
  10. ^ a b Hassard, Jack; Dias, Michael (2013). The Art of Teaching Science: Inquiry and Innovation in Middle School and High School. Oxon: Routledge. ISBN 978-1-135-88999-9.
  11. ^ Vygotsky, L. S. & Luria, A., (1930) Tool and symbol in child development marxists.org
  12. ^ Lev Vygotsky (1931) marxists.org: Adolescent Pedagogy The development of thinking and concept formation in adolescence
  13. ^ Vygotsky, L. S. (1931–32) On Spinoza marxists.org
  14. ^
    • Vygotsky: "...My intellect has been shaped under the sign of Spinoza's words, and it has tried not to be astounded, not to laugh, not to cry, but to understand." (in his dissertation thesis Psychology of Art) [original in Russian]
    • Vygotsky: "...From the great creations of Spinoza, as from distant stars, light takes several centuries to reach us. Only the psychology of the future will be able to realize the ideas of Spinoza." [original in Russian]
    • Vygotsky: "...We cannot help but note that we have come to the same understanding of freedom and self-control that Spinoza developed in his 'Ethics'." (Self-Control, 1931) [original in Russian]
    • Vygotsky: "...Spinoza's teaching contains specifically what is in neither of the two parts into which contemporary psychology of emotions has disintegrated: the unity of the causal explanation and the problem of the vital significance of human passions, the unity of descriptive and explanatory psychology of feelings. For this reason, Spinoza is closely connected with the most vital, the most critical news of the day for contemporary psychology of emotions, news of the day which prevails in it, determining the paroxysm of crisis that envelops it. The problems of Spinoza await their solution, without which tomorrow's day in our psychology is impossible." (The Teaching about Emotions, 1932) [original in Russian]
  15. ^ Kline, G.L. (ed.): Spinoza in Soviet Philosophy. A Series of Essays Selected and Translated and with an Introduction. (New York: The Humanities Press, 1952)
  16. ^ Maidansky, A. (2003). "The Russian Spinozists". Studies in East European Thought. 5 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1023/A:1024066221394. S2CID 169586377.
  17. ^ Derry, J. Vygotsky, Philosophy and Education. (Chichester, West Sussex: Wiley Blackwell, 2013). Derry (2013, p. 85): "Vygotsky's understanding of free will derives from Spinoza. His work is peppered with references to Spinoza and, according to his childhood friend Semyon Dobkin, Spinoza was his favourite philosopher".
  18. ^ Secker, M.: Spinoza's Theory of Emotion in Relation to Vygotsky's Psychology and Damasio's Neuroscience. (Ph.D. diss., University of East Anglia, 2014)
  19. ^ Roth, W.-M.: The Mathematics of Mathematics: Thinking with the Late, Spinozist Vygotsky. (Rotterdam: Sense Publishers, 2017)
  20. ^ Roth, W.-M.; Jornet, A.: Understanding Educational Psychology: A Late Vygotskian, Spinozist Approach. (Dordrecht: Springer, 2017)
  21. ^ Jornet, A.; Cole, M. (2018). "Introduction to symposium on Vygotsky and Spinoza". Mind, Culture, and Activity. 25 (4): 340–345. doi:10.1080/10749039.2018.1538379. S2CID 145032408.. Jornet & Cole (2018): "It has been known since the publication of Thought and Language in English that at the end of his life, Vygotsky turned to the ideas of Spinoza to overcome what he considered the shortcomings of his earlier theoretical ideas, bringing emotion to center stage in the process of development. Recent scholarship has made it clear that Spinoza was important from the beginning of Vygotsky's career. His doctoral thesis, The Psychology of Art, opens with a quotation from Spinoza, and years later Leont'ev (1997) made it clear in his introduction to Vygotsky's collected works that Vygotsky's interest in the philosophy of Spinoza began as early as his student years, and "would remain his favorite thinker for the rest of his life". Spinoza's lifelong influence on Vygotsky, however, has remained a relatively unexplored issue."
  22. ^ Haggbloom, S.J.; Warnick, R.; Warnick, J.E.; Jones, V.K.; Yarbrough, G.L.; Russell, T.M.; Borecky, C.M.; McGahhey, R.; Powell III, J.L.; Beavers, J.; Monte, E. (2002). "The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century". Review of General Psychology. 6 (2): 139–152. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.586.1913. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139. S2CID 145668721.
  23. ^ Pound, L. (2019). How Children Learn (New ed.). London: Andrews UK Limited. p. 51. ISBN 978-1-909280-73-1.
  24. ^ Yasnitsky, A. (2018). Vygotsky: An Intellectual Biography. London and New York: Routledge BOOK PREVIEW
  25. ^ Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). Understanding Vygotsky. A quest for synthesis. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
  26. ^ Nutbrown, Cathy; Clough, Peter; Selbie, Philip (2008). Early Childhood Education: History, Philosophy and Experience. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. p. 57. ISBN 978-1-4129-4497-7.
  27. ^ van der Veer, R. & Zavershneva, E. (2011). To Moscow with Love: Partial Reconstruction of Vygotsky's Trip to London. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science 45(4), 458–474: PDF, pdf
  28. ^ Завершнева Е.Ю. «Ключ к психологии человека»: комментарии к блокноту Л.С. Выготского из больницы «Захарьино» (1926 г.) // Вопр. психол. 2009. №3. С. 123—141
  29. ^ Zavershneva, E. "The Key to Human Psychology". Commentary on L.S. Vygotsky's Notebook from the Zakharino Hospital (1926). Journal of Russian and East European Psychology 50(4), July–August 2012
  30. ^ a b Zavershneva, E. 2009. Issledovanie rukopisi L.S. Vygotskogo "Istoricheskii smysl psikhologicheskogo krizisa" [Investigation of the original of Vygotsky's manuscript "Historical meaning of crisis in psychology"]. Voprosy psikhologii (6):119-137.
  31. ^ a b Завершнева Е.Ю. Исследование рукописи Л.С. Выготского "Исторический смысл психологического кризиса" // Вопросы психологии, 2009. №6, с. 119 - 138.
  32. ^ a b Zavershneva, E. Investigating the Manuscript of L.S. Vygotsky's "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology". Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 50(4), July–August 2012
  33. ^ a b Завершнева Е.Ю., Осипов М.Е. Основные поправки к тексту «Исторический смысл психологического кризиса», опубликованному в 1982 г. в собрании сочинений Л.С. Выготского // Вопросы психологии, 2010. №1. С. 92—103
  34. ^ E. Iu. Zavershneva and M. E. Osipov. Primary Changes to the Version of "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology" Published in the Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, vol. 50(4), July–August 2012
  35. ^ a b Kozulin, Alex. 1986. "Vygotsky in Context" in Vygotsky L. "Thought and Language", MIT Press. pp. xi - lvii
  36. ^ Cf. self-criticism of 1929: "I am revising the s[econd] part of "monkey"[i.e., the book Ape, primitive, and child]. Alas! The f[irst] chapter is written wholly according to the Freudianists [...]; then the impenetrable Piaget is turned into an absolute beyond all measure; instrument and sign are mixed together even more, and so on and so forth. This is not the fault of A. R. [Luria] personally, but of the entire "epoch" of our thinking. We need to put a stop to this unrelentingly. [...] Let there be the most rigorous, monastic regime of thought; ideological seclusion, if necessary. And let us demand the same of others. Let us explain that studying cultural psychology is no joke, not something to do at odd moments or among other things, and not grounds for every new person's own conjectures". In: Vygotsky, L. S. (2007). "Letters to students and colleagues". Journal of Russian and East European Psychology. 45 (2): 11–60. doi:10.2753/RPO1061-0405450201. S2CID 146444813.
  37. ^ Cf. self-criticism of 1930: "In the process of development, and in the historical development in particular, it is not so much the functions which change (these we mistakenly studies before). Their structure and the system of their development remain the same. What is changed and modified are rather the relationships, the links between the functions. New constellations emerge which were unknown in the preceding stage". In: Vygotsky, L. S. (1930/1997). On psychological systems. In R. W. Rieber & J. Wollock (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 3. Problems of the Theory and History of Psychology, pp. 91-108). New York: Plenum Press
  38. ^ From the letter to A. R. Luria, from Moscow, June 12, 1931: "I am still beset with thousands of petty chores. The fruitlessness of what I do greatly distresses me. My scientific thinking is going off into the realm of fantasy, and I cannot think things through in a realistic way to the end. Nothing is going right: I am doing the wrong things, writing the wrong things, saying the wrong things. A fundamental reorganization is called for—and this time I am going to carry it out." In: Vygotsky, L. S. (2007). Letters to students and colleagues. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 45(2), 11-60. doi:10.2753/RPO1061-0405450201, p. 36
  39. ^ Vygotsky, archival document of mid-1932 titled "Consciousness without word": "Our deficiency is not a deficiency of facts, but the untenability of the theory: in the analysis of our crisis this is the main difficulty, but not a departure from facts. This is contra A[.]N.[Leontiev.] Consequently: salvation is not in the facts but in the theory. We introduced the systemic point of view too late... Now I understand all this more deeply" (Zavershneva, 2010b, p. 54)
  40. ^ Vygotsky in his presentation of December 1932, a year and half before his death: "1. The necessity of a new stage of inquiry does not stem from the fact that a new thought has occurred to me or a new idea has caught my interest, but from the necessity of developing the research itself—new facts prod me into searching for new and more intricate explanations. The narrowness, bias, and schematism of the old mindset led us to the wrong assessment of the essential principles that we mistook for the secondary ones: interfunctional connections. We focused attention on the sign (on the tool) to the detriment of the operation with it, representing it as something simple, which goes through three phases: magical—external—internal. But the knot is external and the teenager's diary is external. Hence we have a sea of poorly explained facts and a desire to delve more deeply into the facts, i.e., to evaluate them theoretically in a different way. 2. The higher and lower functions are not constructed in two tiers: their number and names do not match. But our previous understanding was not right, either [, according to which] a higher function is the mastery of the lower ([e.g.,] voluntary attention is the subordination to it of involuntary attention) because this means exactly—in two tiers". Vygotsky's record titled "Symposium, December 4, 1932", see in Zavershneva, E. 2010b. "The Vygotsky Family Archive: New Findings. Notebooks, Notes, and Scientific Journals of L.S. Vygotsky (1912–1934)". Journal of the Russian and East European Psychology 48 (1):34-60, pp. 41-42
  41. ^ Kurt, Dr Serhat (October 22, 2022). "Vygotsky's Theories and How to Incorporate Vygotsky's Theories in The Classroom". Education Library. Retrieved October 23, 2022.
  42. ^ McLeod, Saul (August 5, 2019). "Vygotsky | Simply Psychology". Archived from the original on August 5, 2019. Retrieved September 1, 2020.
  43. ^ Fraser, J.; Yasnitsky, A. (2015). "Deconstructing Vygotsky's Victimization Narrative: A Re-Examination of the "Stalinist Suppression" of Vygotskian Theory" (PDF). History of the Human Sciences. 28 (2): 128–153. doi:10.1177/0952695114560200. S2CID 4934828.
  44. ^ The last line of the notebook entry, from Shakespeare's Hamlet, 'The rest is silence', was also the last line of Vygotsky's first publication (1915), 'The Tragedy of Hamlet, Prince of Denmark', repr. as ch. 8 in Vygotsky, The Psychology of Art (1925).
  45. ^ a b Kozulin, A. (1986). "The concept of activity in Soviet psychology: Vygotsky, his disciples and critics". American Psychologist. 41 (3): 264–274. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.3.264.
  46. ^ Vassilieva, J. (2010). "Russian psychology at the turn of the 21st century and post-Soviet reforms in the humanities disciplines". History of Psychology. 13 (2): 138–159. doi:10.1037/a0019270. PMID 20533768.
  47. ^ :Interview with Laszlo Garai on the Activity Theory of Alexis Leontiev and his own Theory of Social Identity as referred to the meta-theory of Lev Vygotsky. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology 2012; vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 50–64.
  48. ^ Vasileva, Olga; Balyasnikova, Natalia (August 7, 2019). "(Re)Introducing Vygotsky's Thought: From Historical Overview to Contemporary Psychology". Frontiers in Psychology. 10: 1515. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01515. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 6692430. PMID 31447717.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  49. ^ Marginson, Simon; Dang, Thi Kim Anh (January 2, 2017). "Vygotsky's sociocultural theory in the context of globalization". Asia Pacific Journal of Education. 37 (1): 116–129. doi:10.1080/02188791.2016.1216827. ISSN 0218-8791.
  50. ^ Roth, W.M.; Lee, J.Y. (June 2007). ""Vygotsky's Neglected Legacy": Cultural Historical Activity Theory" (PDF). Review of Educational Research. 77 (2): 186–232. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.584.7175. doi:10.3102/0034654306298273. S2CID 12099538.
  51. ^ a b Toomela, A (2000). "Activity theory is a dead end for cultural-historical psychology". Culture & Psychology. 6 (3): 353–364. doi:10.1177/1354067x0063005. S2CID 143980608.
  52. ^ a b Toomela, A (2008). "Activity theory is a dead end for methodological thinking in cultural psychology too". Culture & Psychology. 14 (3): 289–303. doi:10.1177/1354067x08088558. S2CID 144330215.
  53. ^ Wood, D. J.; Bruner, J. S.; Ross, G. (1976). "The role of tutoring in problem solving" (PDF). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 17 (2): 89–100. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x. PMID 932126.
  54. ^ Cf. Valsiner, J. (1988). Developmental psychology in the Soviet Union. Brighton, Sussex: Harvester Press, p. 117: Present-day psychologists’ interest in Vygotsky's thinking is indeed paradoxical. On the one hand, his writings seem increasingly popular among developmental psychologists in Europe and North America. On the other hand, however, careful analyses and thorough understanding of the background of Vygotsky's ideas is rare... Vygotsky seems to be increasingly well known in international psychology, while remaining little understood. The roots of his thinking in international philosophical and psychological discourse remain largely hidden. His ideas have rarely been developed further, along either theoretical or empirical lines.
  55. ^ Simon, J (1987). "Vygotsky and the Vygotskians". American Journal of Education. 95 (4): 609–613. doi:10.1086/444328. S2CID 142282282.
  56. ^ Van der Veer, R., and J. Valsiner. 1991. Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. Oxford: Blackwell, p. 1
  57. ^ Cazden, C. B. 1996. Selective traditions: Readings of Vygotsky in writing pedagogy. In Child discourse and social learning: An interdisciplinary perspective, edited by D. Hicks, 165-186. New York: Cambridge University Press
  58. ^ Palincsar, A. S. (1998). "Keeping the metaphor of scaffolding fresh - a response to C. Addison Stone's "The metaphor of scaffolding: Its utility for the field of learning disabilities" (PDF). Journal of Learning Disabilities. 31 (4): 370–373. doi:10.1177/002221949803100406. hdl:2027.42/68637. PMID 9666613. S2CID 26881323.
  59. ^ Mercer, N.; Fisher, E. (1992). "How do teachers help children to learn? An analysis of teacher's interventions in compter-based activities". Learning and Instruction. 2 (339–355): 342. doi:10.1016/0959-4752(92)90022-E.
  60. ^ Valsiner, J., & Van der Veer, R. (1993). The encoding of distance: The concept of the zone of proximal development and its interpretations. In R. R. Cocking & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), The development and meaning of psychological distance (pp. 35-62). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  61. ^ Valsiner, J., & van der Veer, R. (2014). Encountering the border: Vygotsky's zona blizaishego razvitya and its implications for theory of development. In A. Yasnitsky, R. van der Veer, & M. Ferrari (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology. (pp. 148-174). Cambridge University Press.
  62. ^ Cf. Valsiner, J., and R. Van der Veer (2000). The social mind: Construction of the idea. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, p.4: It is often an open question as to what functions such declarations can have in science. From a position of in-depth analysis, such statements seem merely to be stating the obvious (compared with the statements like the rain is wet or the rich are affluent). And yet, such general claims about the sociality of the human psyche are made with remarkable vigour and repetitiveness
  63. ^ Gillen, J (2000). "Versions of Vygotsky". British Journal of Educational Studies. 48 (2): 183–98. doi:10.1111/1467-8527.t01-1-00141. S2CID 8639392.
  64. ^ van der Veer, R. 2008. Multiple readings of Vygotsky. In The transformation of learning: Advances in cultural-historical activity theory, edited by B. van Oers, W. Wardekker, E. Elbers and R. van der Veer, 20-37. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  65. ^ Gredler, M. E. (2007). "Of cabbages and kings: Concepts and inferences curiously attributed to Lev Vygotsky (Commentary on McVee, Dunsmore, and Gavelek, 2005)". Review of Educational Research. 77 (2): 233–238. doi:10.3102/0034654306298270. S2CID 145115751.
  66. ^ Gredler, M. E.; Schields, C. S. (2004). "Does no one read Vygotsky's words? Commentary on Glassman". Educational Researcher. 33 (2): 21–25. doi:10.3102/0013189x033002021. S2CID 145203451.
  67. ^ Gredler, M.E. (2012). "Understanding Vygotsky for the classroom: Is it too late?". Educational Psychology Review. 24 (1): 113–131. doi:10.1007/s10648-011-9183-6. S2CID 144839041.
  68. ^ Rowlands, S. Turning Vygotsky on His Head: Vygotsky's "Scientifically Based Method" and the Socioculturalist's "Social Other". Science & Education, vol. 9, Issue 6, p.537-575
  69. ^ Miller, R. (2011). Vygotsky in perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  70. ^ Smagorinsky, P. 2011. Vygotsky and Literacy Research: A Methodological Framework. Rotterdam & Boston: Sense.
  71. ^ Lambert, E. Beverley (2000). "Questioning Vygotsky's 'Theory' of Play". Early Child Development and Care. 160 (1): 25–31. doi:10.1080/0030443001600103. S2CID 144872498.
  72. ^ Zhang, R. [張芮菡]. (2013). Rethinking Vygotsky : a critical reading of Vygotsky's cultural-historical theory and its appropriation in contemporary scholarship. (Thesis). University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong SAR. Retrieved from [1]
  73. ^ Zhang, R. (2018). Rethinking Vygotsky: A Critical reading of the semiotics in Vygotsky's cultural-historical theory. In Yasnitsky, A. (Ed.) Questioning Vygotsky's Legacy: Scientific Psychology or Heroic Cult. New York & London: Routledge
  74. ^ Newman, S. (2018). Vygotsky, Wittgenstein, and sociocultural theory. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 48(3), 350-368
  75. ^ Yasnitsky, A., van der Veer, R., Aguilar, E. & García, L.N. (Eds.) (2016). Vygotski revisitado: una historia crítica de su contexto y legado Archived August 17, 2018, at the Wayback Machine. Buenos Aires: Miño y Dávila Editores
  76. ^ Maidansky, A (2020). "Revisionist revolution in Vygotsky studies" (PDF). Studies in East European Thought. 72: 89–95. doi:10.1007/s11212-020-09359-1. S2CID 216343144.
  77. ^ Yasnitsky, A. (2018). Vygotsky's Marxism: A 21st Century Leftist Bolshevik Critique («Le marxisme de Vygotski: Le 21e siecle critique gauchiste bolchevique»). Discussion paper presented on June 22, 2018, at the 7e Seminaire international Vygotski Archived September 11, 2018, at the Wayback Machine held at the Universite de Geneve, June 20–22, 2018, Geneva, Switzerland.
  78. ^ Veer, R., van der (1997). Translator's foreword and acknowledgments. In: Rieber, R.W. & Wollock, J. (Eds.), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Vol.3. Problems of the theory and history of psychology, pp. v-vi. New York-London: Plenum Press
  79. ^ van der Veer, R. (1998). Book review: L. S. Vygotsky. Educational Psychology. Robert Silverman, Trans. Boca Raton FL: St. Lucie Press, 1997. 374 pp. $39.95. ISBN 1-878205-15-3. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences Vol. 34(4), 430–431
  80. ^ a b van der Veer, R.; Yasnitsky, A. (2011). "Vygotsky in English: What Still Needs to Be Done". Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. 45 (4): 475–493. doi:10.1007/s12124-011-9172-9. PMC 3181411. PMID 21626141.
  81. ^ Zavershneva, E. 2007. "Put' k svobode" (K publikatsii materialov iz semejnogo arkhiva L.S. Vygotskogo) ["The road to freedom" (To the publication of the materials from the family archive of L.S. Vygotsky)]. Novoe literaturnoe obozrenie 85 (5):67-90
  82. ^ Zavershneva, E. 2008a. Zapisnye knizhki, zametki, nauchnye dnevniki L.S. Vygotskogo: rezul'taty issledovaniya semejnogo arkhiva [Notebooks, notes, scientific diaries of L.S. Vygotsky: the results of the investigation of the family archive, part 1]. Voprosy psikhologii (1):132—145.
  83. ^ Zavershneva, E. 2008b. Zapisnye knizhki, zametki, nauchnye dnevniki L.S. Vygotskogo: rezul'taty issledovaniya semejnogo arkhiva [Notebooks, notes, scientific diaries of L.S. Vygotsky: the results of the investigation of the family archive, part 2]. Voprosy psikhologii (2):120-136.
  84. ^ Zavershneva, E (2010a). "The Vygotsky Family Archive (1912–1934). New Findings". Journal of Russian and East European Psychology. 48 (1): 14–33. doi:10.2753/rpo1061-0405480101. S2CID 143118053.
  85. ^ Zavershneva, E (2010b). "The Vygotsky Family Archive: New Findings. Notebooks, Notes, and Scientific Journals of L.S. Vygotsky (1912–1934)". Journal of Russian and East European Psychology. 48 (1): 34–60. doi:10.2753/rpo1061-0405480102. S2CID 142733451.
  86. ^ Zavershneva, E (2010c). ""The Way to Freedom" (On the Publication of Documents from the Family Archive of Lev Vygotsky)". Journal of Russian and East European Psychology. 48 (1): 61–90. doi:10.2753/rpo1061-0405480103. S2CID 149007217.
  87. ^ Zavershneva, E. 2012a. Evreiskii vopros v neopublikovannykh rukopisiakh L.S. Vygotskogo [Jewish question in the unpublished manuscripts of L.S. Vygotsky]. Voprosy psikhologii (2):79-99.
  88. ^ Zavershneva, E. 2012. "The Key to Human Psychology". Commentary on L.S. Vygotsky's Notebook from the Zakharino Hospital (1926)" Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012
  89. ^ Yasnitsky, A., van der Veer, R., & Ferrari, M. (Eds.) (2014). The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  90. ^ Keiler, P. (2012). «Cultural-Historical Theory» and «Cultural-Historical School»: From Myth (Back) to Reality // PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 5 (1), 1—33 Archived February 4, 2013, at the Wayback Machine
  91. ^ Кайлер, П. «Культурно-историческая теория» и «культурно-историческая школа»: От мифа (обратно) к реальности // Психологический журнал Международного университета природы, общества и человека «Дубна», ibid., с. 34—46 Archived February 4, 2013, at the Wayback Machine (in Russian)
  92. ^ Keiler, P. (2018). A history of the social construction of the “cultural-historical”. In Yasnitsky, A. (Ed.) Questioning Vygotsky's Legacy: Scientific Psychology or Heroic Cult. New York & London: Routledge
  93. ^ Tudge, J. 1999. Discovering Vygotsky: A Historical and Developmental Approach to His Theory. In Undiscovered Vygotsky. Etudes on the Pre-history of Cultural-Historical Psychology, ed. N. Veresov, pp. 10–17. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.
  94. ^ Chaiklin, Seth. 2003. The Zone of Proximal Development in Vygotsky's Analysis of Learning and Instruction. In Vygotsky's Educational Theory in Cultural Context, ed. A. Kozulin, V.S. Ageyev, S.M. Miller, and B. Gindis, pp. 39–64. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  95. ^ Keiler, P (2012). ""Cultural-Historical Theory" and "Cultural-Historical School": From Myth (Back) to Reality" (PDF). PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal. 5 (1): 1–33. Archived from the original (PDF) on September 21, 2013.
  96. ^ Ясницкий, А. (2011). ""Когда б вы знали, из какого сора...": К определению состава и хронологии создания основных работ Выготского" [“When only you knew what rubbish ...”: On defining the composition and chronology of the creation of Vygotsky's main works] (PDF). PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal (in Russian) (4): 1–52. ISSN 2076-7099. Archived (PDF) from the original on November 2, 2013.
    PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal (4) Archived December 20, 2013, at the Wayback Machine
  97. ^ E. Iu. Zavershneva and M.E. Osipov. Primary Changes to the Version of "The Historical Meaning of the Crisis in Psychology" Published in the Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, vol. 50, no. 4, July–August 2012
  98. ^ Tulviste, P. 1987. Shestitomnoe izdanie trudov L.S. Vygotskogo [Six-volume edition of L.S. Vygotsky's oeuvre]. Voprosy psikhologii, no. 2, pp. 170–73.
  99. ^ Mecacci, L. 1990. "Edizioni e traduzioni di Pensiero e linguaggio." In Vygotskij, L.S. Pensiero e Linguaggio. Ricerche psicologiche, pp. xv–xviii. Roma: Laterza.
  100. ^ Brushlinskii, A. V. (1996). Pervye utochneniya tekstov L.S. Vygotskogo [First clarifications of L.S. Vygotsky's published texts]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 17, 19–25
  101. ^ Peshkov, I. V. (1999). Tekstologicheskij kommentarij [Textological commentary]. In L. S. Vygotskii, Thinking and speech’ (pp. 339). Moscow: Labirint.
  102. ^ Peshkov, I. V. (2008). Tsenzura stilya ne rekomenduetsya [Style censorship is not recommended]. In L. S. Vygotskii (Ed.), Psikhologiya iskusstva (pp. 338–340). Moscow: Labirint
  103. ^ Kellogg, D. & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). The differences between the Russian and English texts of Tool and Symbol in Child Development. Supplementary and analytic materials Archived December 20, 2013, at the Wayback Machine. PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 4(4) Archived December 20, 2013, at the Wayback Machine, 98-158
  104. ^ Mecacci, L., & Yasnitsky, A. (2011). Editorial Changes in the Three Russian Editions of Vygotsky's Thinking and Speech (1934, 1956, 1982): Towards Authoritative and Ultimate English Translation of the Book Archived December 20, 2013, at the Wayback Machine. PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 4(4) Archived December 20, 2013, at the Wayback Machine, 159-187
  105. ^ Goldberg E. The wisdom paradox: How your mind can grow stronger as your brain grows older. New York: Gotham, 2005, p. 99
  106. ^ Rieber, R., & Robinson, D. (2004). Preface. In R. W. Rieber & D. K. Robinson (Eds.), The essential Vygotsky (pp. xiii-xvii). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
  107. ^ Goldberg, Е. (2012). "Thank you for sharing this fascinating material - very interesting" Archived September 21, 2013, at the Wayback Machine // PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 5 (1) Archived February 4, 2013, at the Wayback Machine, 118-120
  108. ^ Cole, M. (2012). Comments on prior Comments Archived September 21, 2013, at the Wayback Machine // PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 5 (1) Archived February 4, 2013, at the Wayback Machine, 124-127
  109. ^ van der Veer, R. (2012). Rukopisi ne goryat or do they? Archived September 22, 2013, at the Wayback Machine // PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 5 (1), 133-138 Archived February 4, 2013, at the Wayback Machine
  110. ^ Veer, R., van der (2010). Vygotsky in English: What still needs to be done. Website for International Cultural Historical Studies [permanent dead link])
  111. ^ Yasnitsky, A. (2012). The Complete Works of L.S. Vygotsky: PsyAnima Complete Vygotsky project Archived March 19, 2013, at the Wayback Machine. PsyAnima, Dubna Psychological Journal, 5(3), 144-148

Further reading

Primary

Secondary

External links